肝脏b超能检查出什么| 87年属什么的| dsa是什么检查| 脂蛋白是什么意思| 卤牛肉用什么部位| 宫颈炎有什么症状表现| 胆囊炎有什么症状表现| 9月14号是什么星座| 冒昧打扰是什么意思| 腮腺炎挂什么科| 960万平方千米是指我国的什么| 阎王叫什么名字| 甲状腺结节对身体有什么影响| 权衡利弊的意思是什么| 开放性神经管缺陷是什么意思| 红豆吃多了有什么坏处| m指的是什么| 狗跟什么生肖最配| 保家仙都有什么仙| 儿童乳房发育挂什么科| hdr是什么拍照功能| 身份证后四位代表什么| 鹿加几念什么| 灰蓝色是什么颜色| 放疗和化疗有什么区别| 梦见亲人死了是什么意思| 形态各异的异是什么意思| 青梅是什么水果| 三姓家奴是什么意思| 羊跟什么生肖最配| 对节木是什么树| 昏厥是什么意思| gp是什么意思| abo是什么| 跳蚤为什么只咬一个人| 痛经什么原因引起的| 呼吸道感染吃什么药| 劳作是什么意思| 一个虫一个夫念什么| mdt是什么意思| ng什么意思| 耳朵会动的人说明什么| 抱持是什么意思| 纵隔是什么意思| 早餐吃什么| 疱疹不能吃什么食物| 做梦梦到钱是什么预兆| 检查前列腺做什么检查| 低密度脂蛋白偏高是什么意思| 实诚是什么意思| 国家为什么不承认鬼神| 阴茎进入阴道是什么感觉| 淡奶油能做什么| 艾灰有什么作用和功效| 嘴唇溃疡是什么原因| 宫颈癌前期有什么症状| 什么大| 起酥油是什么东西| 什么是脑死亡| 摄取是什么意思| 智商105是什么水平| 嗜碱性粒细胞比率偏高说明什么| 7月出生是什么星座| 疯狂动物城闪电是什么动物| 什么情况下需做肠镜| 化干戈为玉帛是什么意思| 前列腺炎有什么征兆| 学生近视配什么镜片好| 什么情况下需要做活检| 殚精竭虑是什么意思| 灰指甲有什么症状| 后期是什么意思啊| 为什么女生会来月经| 糯米粉可以做什么好吃的| 喉炎是什么原因引起的| 2001年属蛇五行属什么| 微博id是什么| ol是什么意思| 见地是什么意思| pe医学上是什么意思| 吃饭掉筷子有什么预兆| 每天早上起床口苦是什么原因| 富硒是什么意思| 蒙羞是什么意思| 高利贷是什么意思| 肺活量不足是什么症状| 1985年属牛是什么命| 下焦不通吃什么中成药| 部队指导员是什么级别| a型熊猫血是什么血型| rem是什么意思| 等离子是什么| 两肋胀满闷胀是什么病| 肝占位是什么意思| 淡盐水有什么作用和功效| 狐臭是什么引起的| 糖类抗原125偏高是什么原因| 本科毕业证是什么颜色| 长高吃什么| msm是什么意思| 太平果是什么水果| 王大治与董洁什么关系| 思源名字的寓意是什么| 泥石流是什么| 吃深海鱼油有什么好处和坏处| 今天什么冲什么| 蔓越莓有什么功效| 湿疹是什么| dob是什么意思| 你的美丽让你带走是什么歌| 晚上喝什么茶有助于睡眠| 清炖排骨汤放什么调料| 乳腺小叶增生是什么意思| 生姜什么时候种植最合适| 天才是指什么生肖| 什么药可以当饭吃| 吃b族维生素有什么好处| 闰六月给父母买什么| 冬占生男是什么意思| 热爱是什么意思| 多酚是什么| 什么如生| 早上起床喉咙有痰是什么原因| 花甲是什么| 一个鸟一个衣是什么字| 内膜厚吃什么掉内膜| 头晕出汗是什么原因| 压力山大什么意思| 清洁度1度是什么意思| 8月8号什么星座| xg是什么意思| 颈部多发淋巴结是什么意思| 切除一侧输卵管对女性有什么影响| 下巴长硬包是什么原因| 助听器什么牌子最好| 发髻是什么意思| 冰室是什么意思| 胰腺检查挂什么科| 什么是碱性磷酸酶高怎么回事| 冬菜是什么菜| 吃维生素b2有什么好处| 1207是什么星座| 脾胃虚弱吃什么蔬菜| 信球什么意思| 简历照片用什么底色| 什么叫捞女| 重阳节又称什么节| 一个山一个空念什么| tj什么意思| 三聚磷酸钠是什么| 普洱茶什么季节喝好| 气是什么意思| 红色配什么颜色| 1994属什么生肖| 33年属什么生肖| 没有鱼鳞的鱼是什么鱼| 塘鲺是什么鱼| 纤维化是什么意思| snr是什么意思| 白头发吃什么维生素能变黑| 烤冷面是什么做的| 北京摇号什么时候开始的| 情窦初开是什么意思| 两胸中间疼是什么原因| 青龙白虎是什么意思| 衣柜放什么代替樟脑丸| 小猫咪能吃什么| 二月是什么星座| 1991年五行属什么| 子不孝父之过下一句是什么| 什么叫湿疹| 呕吐发烧吃什么药| 眼皮红肿是什么原因引起的| 建兰什么时候开花| 马齿笕有什么功效| 布五行属什么| hib是什么疫苗| 龟头炎用什么药好| 7月25号是什么星座| 得糖尿病的原因是什么| 二便是什么意思| 纨绔子弟是什么意思| 什么叫放疗| 胆囊壁厚是什么意思| 左手麻木什么原因| 吃什么水果对嗓子好| 胃酸反流是什么原因造成| 打升白针有什么副作用| 牙囊肿是什么病严重吗| 麻子是什么意思| 恰如其分是什么意思| bpm是什么| cd3cd4cd8都代表什么| 杭州有什么好吃的| 脂肪瘤看什么科| 手心朝上是什么意思| 捂脸表情什么意思| 女人带貔貅有什么讲究| 反胃吃什么药| 悟空是什么意思| 吼不住是什么意思| 丙二醇是什么| 肚脐眼是什么穴位| 羽毛球拍u是什么意思| 中国反导弹系统叫什么| 沉默寡言是什么意思| 单纯是什么意思| 二郎神是什么生肖| 高回声是什么意思| 走路脚后跟疼是什么原因| 大腿外侧疼痛是什么原因| 脂溢性皮炎用什么药| 88年的龙是什么命| 草字头占读什么| 胸骨突出是什么原因| 摩什么接什么| 啪啪啪什么感觉| 柔软的近义词是什么| 绍兴有什么大学| 透析病人磷高了吃什么降磷| 什么解酒快| 菌群失调是什么意思| 11月10号是什么星座| 人的舌头有什么作用| 诺言背叛诺言是什么歌| 奥美拉唑什么时候吃最好| 吃知柏地黄丸有什么副作用| 251什么意思| 冰释前嫌的释是什么意思| 白细胞偏低是什么意思| 今天什么生肖最旺| 经常偏头疼是什么原因| 百里挑一是什么生肖| 夜盲症缺什么维生素| 九月是什么星座的| 舌头上火了吃什么降火| 班别是什么意思| 抗氧化是什么意思| em是什么意思| 什么叫丹毒| 西楼是什么意思| 梅毒是什么样的| 身上老出汗是什么原因引起的| soe咖啡是什么意思| 空代表什么生肖| 二人世界是什么意思| 扫把和什么是一套的| 眼睛长结石是什么原因引起的| 玩世不恭是什么意思| 脚后跟长痣有什么寓意| 肝郁血瘀吃什么中成药| 拉肚子是什么原因引起的| 5公里25分钟什么水平| 梦见很多蛇是什么意思| 什么是有氧运动和无氧运动| 舌苔发黄什么原因| 传单是什么病| 打扮的意思是什么| 卡号是什么| 吃什么可以快速美白| 肛塞有什么作用| 总是饿是什么原因| 本帮菜是什么意思| 志趣相投是什么意思| 百度Jump to content

新春走基层 在记录变与不变中触摸时代脉搏

From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia
(Redirected from Inferotemporal cortex)
Inferior temporal gyrus
Lateral surface of left cerebral hemisphere, viewed from the side. (Inferior temporal gyrus shown in orange.)
Drawing of a cast to illustrate the relations of the brain to the skull. (Inferior temporal gyrus labeled at center, in green section.)
Details
Part ofTemporal lobe
ArteryPosterior cerebral
Identifiers
Latingyrus temporalis inferior
NeuroNames138
NeuroLex IDbirnlex_1577
TA98A14.1.09.148
TA25497
FMA61907
Anatomical terms of neuroanatomy
百度 随后,包括中国外交部、国防部、国台办等在内的几大机构,接连就此发声和亮剑,表示强烈不满和坚决反对。

The inferior temporal gyrus is one of three gyri of the temporal lobe and is located below the middle temporal gyrus, connected behind with the inferior occipital gyrus; it also extends around the infero-lateral border on to the inferior surface of the temporal lobe, where it is limited by the inferior sulcus. This region is one of the higher levels of the ventral stream of visual processing, associated with the representation of objects, places, faces, and colors.[1][2] It may also be involved in face perception,[3] and in the recognition of numbers and words.[4][5]

The inferior temporal gyrus is the anterior region of the temporal lobe located underneath the central temporal sulcus. The primary function of the occipital temporal gyrus – otherwise referenced as IT cortex – is associated with visual stimuli processing, namely visual object recognition, and has been suggested by recent experimental results as the final location of the ventral cortical visual system.[6] The IT cortex in humans is also known as the Inferior Temporal Gyrus since it has been located to a specific region of the human temporal lobe.[7] The IT processes visual stimuli of objects in our field of vision, and is involved with memory and memory recall to identify that object; it is involved with the processing and perception created by visual stimuli amplified in the V1, V2, V3, and V4 regions of the occipital lobe. This region processes the color and form of the object in the visual field and is responsible for producing the "what" from this visual stimuli, or in other words identifying the object based on the color and form of the object and comparing that processed information to stored memories of objects to identify that object.[6]

The IT cortex's neurological significance is not just its contribution to the processing of visual stimuli in object recognition but also has been found to be a vital area with regards to simple processing of the visual field, difficulties with perceptual tasks and spatial awareness, and the location of unique single cells that possibly explain the IT cortex's relation to memory.

Structure

[edit]
Human right cerebral hemisphere. Lateral view (left) and medial view (right). In both images, inferior temporal gyrus labeled at bottom. The areas colored green represent temporal lobe. (Brown is occipital and purple is limbic respectively.)

The temporal lobe is unique to primates. In humans, the IT cortex is more complex than their relative primate counterparts. The human inferior temporal cortex consists of the inferior temporal gyrus, the middle temporal gyrus, and the fusiform gyrus. When looking at the brain laterally – that is from the side and looking at the surface of the temporal lobe – the inferior temporal gyrus is along the bottom portion of the temporal lobe, and is separated from the middle temporal gyrus located directly above by the inferior temporal sulcus. Additionally, some processing of the visual field that corresponds to the ventral stream of visual processing occurs in the lower portion of the superior temporal gyrus closest to the superior temporal sulcus. The medial and ventral view of the brain – meaning looking at the medial surface from below the brain, facing upwards – reveals that the inferior temporal gyrus is separated from the fusiform gyrus by the occipital-temporal sulcus. This human inferior temporal cortex is much more complex than that of other primates: non-human primates have an inferior temporal cortex that is not divided into unique regions such as humans' inferior temporal gyrus, fusiform gyrus, or middle temporal gyrus.[8]

This region of the brain corresponds to the inferior temporal cortex and is responsible for visual object recognition and receives processed visual information. The inferior temporal cortex in primates has specific regions dedicated to processing different visual stimuli processed and organized by the different layers of the striate cortex and extra-striate cortex. The information from the V1 –V5 regions of the geniculate and tectopulvinar pathways are radiated to the IT cortex via the ventral stream: visual information specifically related to the color and form of the visual stimuli. Through comparative research between primates – humans and non-human primates – results indicate that the IT cortex plays a significant role in visual shape processing. This is supported by functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) data collected by researchers comparing this neurological process between humans and macaques.[9]

Function

[edit]

Receiving information

[edit]

The light energy that comes from the rays bouncing off of an object is converted into chemical energy by the cells in the retina of the eye. This chemical energy is then converted into action potentials that are transferred through the optic nerve and across the optic chiasm, where it is first processed by the lateral geniculate nucleus of the thalamus. From there the information is sent to the primary visual cortex, region V1. It then travels from the visual areas in the occipital lobe to the parietal and temporal lobes via two distinct anatomical streams.[10] These two cortical visual systems were classified by Ungerleider and Mishkin (1982, see two-streams hypothesis).[11] One stream travels ventrally to the inferior temporal cortex (from V1 to V2 then through V4 to ITC) while the other travels dorsally to the posterior parietal cortex. They are labeled the "what" and "where" streams, respectively. The Inferior Temporal Cortex receives information from the ventral stream, understandably so, as it is known to be a region essential in recognizing patterns, faces, and objects.[12]

The dorsal stream (green) and ventral stream (purple) originating in the primary visual cortex.

Single-cell function in the inferior temporal gyrus

[edit]

The understanding at the single-cell level of the IT cortex and its role of utilizing memory to identify objects and or process the visual field based on color and form visual information is a relatively recent in neuroscience. Early research indicated that the cellular connections of the temporal lobe to other memory associated areas of the brain – namely the hippocampus, the amygdala, the prefrontal cortex, among others. These cellular connections have recently been found to explain unique elements of memory, suggesting that unique single-cells can be linked to specific unique types and even specific memories. Research into the single-cell understanding of the IT cortex reveals many compelling characteristics of these cells: single-cells with similar selectivity of memory are clustered together across the cortical layers of the IT cortex; the temporal lobe neurons have recently been shown to display learning behaviors and possibly relate to long-term memory; and, cortical memory within the IT cortex is likely to be enhanced over time thanks to the influence of the afferent-neurons of the medial-temporal region.

Further research of the single-cells of the IT cortex suggests that these cells not only have a direct link to the visual system pathway but also are deliberate in the visual stimuli they respond to: in certain cases, the single-cell IT cortex neurons do not initiate responses when spots or slits, namely simple visual stimuli, are present in the visual field; however, when complicated objects are put in place, this initiates a response in the single-cell neurons of the IT cortex. This provides evidence that not only are the single-cell neurons of the IT cortex related in having a unique specific response to visual stimuli but rather that each individual single-cell neuron has a specific response to a specific stimuli. The same study also reveals how the magnitude of the response of these single-cell neurons of the IT cortex do not change due to color and size but are only influenced by the shape. This led to even more interesting observations where specific IT neurons have been linked to the recognition of faces and hands. This is very interesting as to the possibility of relating to neurological disorders of prosopagnosia and explaining the complexity and interest in the human hand. Additional research from this study goes into more depth on the role of "face neurons" and "hand neurons" involved in the IT cortex.

The significance of the single-cell function in the IT cortex is that it is another pathway in addition to the lateral geniculate pathway that processes most visual system: this raises questions about how does it benefit our visual information processing in addition to normal visual pathways and what other functional units are involved in additional visual information processing.[13]

Information processing

[edit]

The information for color and form comes from P-cells that receive their information mainly from cones, so they are sensitive to differences in form and color, as opposed to the M-cells that receive information about motion mainly from rods. The neurons in the inferior temporal cortex, also called the inferior temporal visual association cortex, process this information from the P-cells.[14] The neurons in the ITC have several unique properties that offer an explanation as to why this area is essential in recognizing patterns. They only respond to visual stimuli and their receptive fields always include the fovea, which is one of the densest areas of the retina and is responsible for acute central vision. These receptive fields tend to be larger than those in the striate cortex and often extend across the midline to unite the two visual half fields for the first time. IT neurons are selective for shape and/or color of stimulus and are usually more responsive to complex shapes as opposed to simple ones. A small percentage of them are selective for specific parts of the face. Faces and likely other complex shapes are seemingly coded by a sequence of activity across a group of cells, and IT cells can display both short or long-term memory for visual stimuli based on experience.[15]

Object recognition

[edit]

There are a number of regions that work together within the ITC for processing and recognizing the information of "what" something is. In fact, discrete categories of objects are even associated with different regions.

Diagram depicting different regions of the left cerebral hemisphere, fusiform in orange.
Same as above, but parahippocampal gyrus now in orange.
  • The extrastriate body area (EBA) helps differentiate body parts from other objects.
  • And the lateral occipital complex (LOC) is used to determine shapes vs. scrambled stimuli.

[16]

These areas must all work together, as well as with the hippocampus, in order to create an array of understanding of the physical world. The hippocampus is key for storing the memory of what an object is/what it looks like for future use so that it can be compared and contrasted with other objects. Correctly being able to recognize an object is highly dependent on this organized network of brain areas that process, share, and store information. In a study by Denys et al., functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) was used to compare the processing of visual shape between humans and macaques. They found, amongst other things, that there was a degree of overlap between shape and motion sensitive regions of the cortex, but that the overlap was more distinct in humans. This would suggest that the human brain is better evolved for a high level of functioning in a distinct, three-dimensional, visual world.[17]

Clinical significance

[edit]

Prosopagnosia

[edit]

Prosopagnosia, also called face blindness, is a disorder that results in the inability to recognize or discriminate between faces. It can often be associated with other forms of recognition impairment, such as place, car, or emotional recognition.[18] A study conducted by Gross et al. in 1969 found that certain cells were selective for the shape of a monkey hand, and they observed that as the stimulus they provided began to further resemble a monkey hand, those cells became more active. A few years later, in 1972, Gross et al. discovered that certain IT cells were selective for faces. Although it is not conclusive, 'face-selective' IT cortex cells are assumed to play a large role in facial recognition in monkeys.[19] After the extensive research into the result of damage to the IT cortex in monkeys, it is theorized that lesions in the IT gyrus in humans result in prosopagnosia. Rubens and Benson's 1971 study of a subject in life with prosopagnosia reveals that the patient is able to name common objects on visual presentation flawlessly, however she cannot recognize faces. Upon necropsy conducted by Benson et al., it was apparent that a discrete lesion in the right fusiform gyrus, a part of the inferior temporal gyrus, was one of the main causes of the subject's symptoms.[20]

A more in depth observation can be seen with the example of patient L.H. in the study conducted by N.L. Etcoff and colleagues in 1991. This 40-year-old man was involved in an automobile accident when he was 18, which resulted in severe brain injury. Upon recovery, L.H. was unable to recognize or discriminate between faces, or even recognize faces that were familiar to him before the accident. L.H. and other patients with prosopagnosia are often able to live relatively normal and productive lives despite their deficit. L.H. was still able to recognize common objects, subtle differences in shapes, and even age, sex, and "likeability" of faces. However, they use non-facial cues, such as height, hair color, and voice to differentiate between people. Non-invasive brain imaging revealed that L.H.'s prosopagnosia was a result of damage to the right temporal lobe, which contains the inferior temporal gyrus.[21]

Deficits in semantic memory

[edit]

Certain disorders, such as Alzheimer's disease and semantic dementia, are characterized by a patient's inability to integrate semantic memories, which results in patients being unable to form new memories, lacking awareness of time period, as well as lacking other important cognitive processes. Chan et al 2001 conducted a study that used volumetric magnetic resonance imaging to quantify the global and temporal lobe atrophy in semantic dementia and Alzheimer's disease. The subjects were selected and confirmed to be in the middle of the spectrum of their respective disorders clinically, and then further confirmation came from a series of neuropsychological tests given to the subjects. The study treated the inferior temporal cortex and the middle temporal cortex as one and the same, because of the, "often indistinct," border between the gyri.[22]

The study concluded that in Alzheimer's disease, deficits in inferior temporal structures were not the main source of the disease. Rather, atrophy in the entorhinal cortex, amygdala, and hippocampus was prominent in the Alzheimer's inflicted subjects of the study. With respect to semantic dementia, the study concluded that "the middle and inferior temporal gyri [cortices] may play a key role" in semantic memory, and as a result, unfortunately, when these anterior temporal lobe structures are injured, the subject is left with semantic dementia. This information shows how, despite often being grouped in the same category, Alzheimer's disease and semantic dementia are very different diseases, and are characterized by marked differences in the subcortical structures they are associated with.[22]

Cerebral achromatopsia

[edit]
An example of vision in a person with cerebral achromatopsia.

Cerebral achromatopsia is a medical disorder characterized by the inability to perceive color and to achieve satisfactory visual acuity in high light levels. Congenital achromatopsia is characterized the same way, however it is genetic, while Cerebral Achromatopsia occurs as a result of damage to certain parts of the brain. One part of the brain that is particularly integral to color discrimination is the inferior temporal gyrus. A 1995 study conducted by Heywood et al. was meant to highlight the parts of the brain that are important in achromatopsia in monkeys, however, it obviously sheds light on the areas of the brain related to achromatopsia in humans. In the study, one group of monkeys (group AT) received lesions in the temporal lobe anterior to V4 and the other group (group MOT) received lesions to the occipito-temporal area that corresponds in cranial location to the lesion that produces cerebral achromatopsia in humans. The study concluded that group MOT had no impairment of their color vision while the subjects in group AT all had severe impairments to their color vision, consistent with humans diagnosed with cerebral achromatopsia.[23] This study shows that temporal lobe areas anterior to V4, which includes the inferior temporal gyrus, play a large role in patients with Cerebral Achromatopsia.

Additional images

[edit]

See also

[edit]

References

[edit]
  1. ^ Baldauf, D.; Desimone, R. (2025-08-14). "Neural Mechanisms of Object-Based Attention". Science. 344 (6182): 424–427. Bibcode:2014Sci...344..424B. doi:10.1126/science.1247003. ISSN 0036-8075. PMID 24763592. S2CID 34728448.
  2. ^ ROSA LAFER-SOUSA and BEVIL CONWAY (October 20, 2013). "Parallel, multi-stage processing of colors, faces and shapes in macaque inferior temporal cortex". Nature Neuroscience. 16 (12): 1870–1878. doi:10.1038/nn.3555. PMC 3957328. PMID 24141314.
  3. ^ Haxby indicates that a few studies have found face perception in the inferior temporal sulcus, with the majority of sites elsewhere in the brain: p.2, Haxby, et al. (2000) "The distributed human neural system for face perception" Trends in Cognitive Sciences 4 (6) June 2000, 11pp.
  4. ^ BRUCE GOLDMAN (April 16, 2013). "Scientists pinpoint brain's area for numeral recognition". Stanford School of Medicine. Retrieved 2025-08-14.
  5. ^ Poggio, Tomaso; Anselmi, Fabio (23 September 2016). Visual Cortex and Deep Networks. MIT Press. pp. 45–51. ISBN 978-0-262-03472-2.
  6. ^ a b Kolb, B; Whishaw, I. Q. (2014). An Introduction to Brain and Behavior (Fourth ed.). New York, NY: Worth. pp. 282–312.
  7. ^ Gross, C. G. (2008). "Inferior temporal cortex". Scholarpedia. 3 (12): 7294. Bibcode:2008SchpJ...3.7294G. doi:10.4249/scholarpedia.7294.
  8. ^ Pessoa L, Tootell R, Ungerleider LG, Squire LR, Bloom FE, McConnel SK, Roberts JL, Spitzer NC, Zigmond MJ, eds. (2008). "Visual Perception of Objects". Fundamental Neuroscience (Third ed.).[clarification needed]
  9. ^ Denys, Katrien; Wim Vanduffel; Denis Fize; Koen Nelissen; Hendrik Peuskens; David Van Essen; Guy A. Orban (10 March 2004). "The Processing of Visual Shape in the Cerebral Cortex of Human and Nonhuman Primates: A Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Study". The Journal of Neuroscience. 24 (24(10): 2551–2565): 2551–2565. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3569-03.2004. PMC 6729498. PMID 15014131.
  10. ^ Kolb, Bryan; Whishaw, Ian Q. (2014). An Introduction to Brain and Behavior (Fourth ed.). New York, NY: Worth. pp. 282–312.
  11. ^ Mishkin, Mortimer; Ungerleider, Leslie G. (1982). "Two Cortical Visual Systems" (PDF). The MIT Press. {{cite journal}}: Cite journal requires |journal= (help)
  12. ^ Creem, Sarah H.; Proffitt, Dennis R. (2001). "Defining the cortical visual systems: "What", "Where", and "How"" (PDF). Acta Psychologica. 107 (1–3): 43–68. doi:10.1016/s0001-6918(01)00021-x. PMID 11388142. Archived from the original (PDF) on 2025-08-14. Retrieved 2025-08-14.
  13. ^ Gross, C. G. (2007). "Single Neuron Studies of Inferior Temporal Cortex". Neuropsychologia. 46 (3): 841–852. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.11.009. PMID 18155735. S2CID 16008718.
  14. ^ Dragoi, Valentin. "Chapter 15: Visual Processing: Cortical Pathways". Archived from the original on 9 April 2014. Retrieved 12 November 2013.
  15. ^ Gross, Charles (2008). "Inferior temporal cortex". Scholarpedia. 3 (12): 7294. Bibcode:2008SchpJ...3.7294G. doi:10.4249/scholarpedia.7294.
  16. ^ Spiridon, M.; Fischl, B.; Kanwisher, N. (2006). "Location and spatial profile of category-specific regions in human extrastriate cortex". Human Brain Mapping. 27 (1): 77–89. doi:10.1002/hbm.20169. PMC 3264054. PMID 15966002.
  17. ^ Denys; et al. (March 10, 2004). "The Processing of Visual Shape in the Cerebral Cortex of Human and Nonhuman Primates: A Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Study". The Journal of Neuroscience. 24 (10): 2551–2565. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3569-03.2004. PMC 6729498. PMID 15014131.
  18. ^ Nakayama, Ken. "Prosopagnosia Research". The President and Fellows of Harvard College. Retrieved 9 November 2013.
  19. ^ Gross, Charles (29 January 1992). "Representation of Visual Stimuli in Inferior Temporal Cortex" (PDF). Philosophical Transactions: Biological Sciences. Processing the Facial Image. 335 (1273): 3–10. doi:10.1098/rstb.1992.0001. PMID 1348134. Archived from the original (PDF) on 25 December 2013. Retrieved 9 November 2013.
  20. ^ Meadows, J.C. (1974). "The anatomical basis of prosopagnosia". Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry. 37 (5): 489–501. doi:10.1136/jnnp.37.5.489. PMC 494693. PMID 4209556.
  21. ^ Purves D, Augustine GJ, Fitzpatrick D, et al., eds. (2001). "Lesions in the Temporal Association Cortex: Deficits in Recognition". Neuroscience (2nd ed.). Retrieved 11 November 2013.
  22. ^ a b Chan, D; Fox NC; Crum WR; Whitwell JL; Leschziner G; Rossor AM; Stevens JM; Cipolotti L; Rossor MN (April 2001). "Patterns of temporal lobe atrophy in semantic dementia and Alzheimer's disease". Annals of Neurology. 49 (4): 433–42. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.569.8292. doi:10.1002/ana.92. PMID 11310620. S2CID 41627534.
  23. ^ Heywood, C.A.; Gaffan D; Cowey A (1995). "Cerebral Achromatopsia in Monkeys" (PDF). European Journal of Neuroscience. 7 (5): 1064–1073. doi:10.1111/j.1460-9568.1995.tb01093.x. PMID 7613611. S2CID 25787249. Retrieved 11 November 2013.
[edit]
门口放什么植物好风水 腰肌劳损是什么意思 肝脏钙化灶是什么意思 碳化是什么意思 子宫癌筛查做什么检查
玉什么样的好 受用是什么意思 羽毛球拍什么材质的好 预防脑出血吃什么药 潴是什么意思
内膜厚是什么原因 mm什么意思 细菌高是什么原因 为什么会突然吐血 缺钾有什么表现和症状
kksk是什么意思 收缩压低是什么原因 咳嗽喉咙痒吃什么药 申时出生五行缺什么 子字五行属什么
跳楼是什么感觉hcv9jop3ns3r.cn louisvuitton什么牌子hcv7jop5ns3r.cn 善什么甘什么hcv8jop1ns0r.cn 牵牛花什么时候开hcv9jop0ns2r.cn 多普勒超声检查是什么0735v.com
胎儿生物物理评分8分什么意思hcv8jop6ns8r.cn 田螺姑娘是什么意思hcv8jop9ns8r.cn 蓝色的猫是什么品种hcv9jop1ns2r.cn 山对什么hcv8jop9ns9r.cn 吃什么补气hcv8jop1ns3r.cn
脑干出血是什么原因造成的hcv7jop7ns1r.cn progress什么意思hcv9jop5ns4r.cn 星星为什么会眨眼睛hcv9jop6ns1r.cn 养尊处优什么意思hlguo.com 巩膜是什么部位hcv9jop4ns9r.cn
时间的定义是什么hcv9jop4ns9r.cn 大眼角痒是什么原因hcv9jop0ns9r.cn 西洋参补什么hcv9jop5ns1r.cn 什么东西有头无脚hcv9jop3ns1r.cn 限行是什么意思hcv7jop7ns0r.cn
百度