怀孕喝酒会有什么反应| 多吃醋有什么好处和坏处| 临床医学是什么| 氧化剂是什么| 口臭吃什么| 珍珠疹是什么原因引起的| 什么是腺样体| 后背出汗多是什么原因| 女性肠痉挛有什么症状| 三公经费指什么| 高锰酸钾治疗男性什么病| 马失前蹄下一句是什么| 96年什么命| 例假可以吃什么水果| 协警是干什么的| 什么的蘑菇| 嘴无味是什么病的征兆| 塞来昔布是什么药| 什么东西越吃越饿| 什么人适合喝三七粉| oioi是什么牌子| 白露是什么季节| 彩铃是什么意思| 宝宝感冒流鼻涕吃什么药| 福兮祸兮是什么意思| 收阴是什么意思| 子宫肌瘤伴钙化是什么意思| 为什么不能指彩虹| 宫寒是什么意思| 什么样的青蛙| 哈衣是什么意思| 大暑吃什么| 钾是什么东西| 口苦尿黄是什么原因| 热能是什么| 什么是痰湿| 苹果5s什么时候上市的| 每天早上起来口苦是什么原因| 男生为什么会勃起| 低脂高钙牛奶适合什么人群| 胸腺肿瘤是什么病| 无锡有什么好玩的| 郭富城属什么生肖| 身份证拍照穿什么衣服| 鼠和什么生肖最配| 器质性病变是什么意思| 性早熟有什么症状| 稻谷是什么| 莲子有什么作用| 卡马西平是什么药| 伏羲女娲是什么关系| 口腔溃疡用什么药好| 棕色眼睛的人什么血统| 吃百香果有什么好处| 什么值得买怎么用| 什么的鼻子| 锁骨是什么骨| 小暑吃什么| 天天洗头发有什么危害| 带教是什么意思| 海带属于什么植物| 老年人腿脚无力是什么原因| 血小板分布宽度偏低是什么原因| 金字旁成是什么字| 巴宝莉属于什么档次| 大运正官是什么意思| 背痛是什么原因| gtp是什么意思| 血管瘤是什么样子的图| 猪脆肠是什么器官| 解神是什么意思| 什么的夏天| 审美疲劳是什么意思| 尿毒症能吃什么水果| hpmc是什么| 政协副主席是什么级别| 卵巢囊性结构是什么意思| 复方对乙酰氨基酚片是什么药| 射手女喜欢什么样的男生| 慢性肠炎吃什么药调理| 火龙果和香蕉榨汁有什么功效| 蛤蟆吃什么| 吃什么食物补肾最快| 台风什么时候到福建| 梦到蛇是什么意思| 冰释前嫌的释是什么意思| 过期的钙片有什么用途| 朗朗原名叫什么| 吉祥如意是什么意思| 开眼镜店需要什么设备| 甲母痣是什么| 肌层回声均匀是什么意思| 什么蛋不能吃| 卦不走空是什么意思| c1是什么意思| 为什么体检前不能喝水| 痃癖是什么病| 械字号产品是什么意思| 胃溃疡可以吃什么水果| tm是什么| hrd是什么意思| 不遗余力什么意思| 胃病忌什么| 升白细胞的针剂叫什么| 散瞳后需要注意什么| hcc是什么意思| 尿沉渣红细胞高是什么原因| 什么样人不能吃海参| 眼睛胀痛什么原因| qid是什么意思| mssa是什么细菌| 黄体酮有什么作用与功效| 鹅吃什么| 血稠是什么原因引起的| 泥鳅什么人不能吃| 预包装食品指的是什么| 披靡是什么意思| 有什么菜| 喉咙痛是什么原因引起的| 不是月经期出血是什么原因| 郑中基为什么叫太子基| 精湛是什么意思| 头发稀少是什么原因| 为什么啊| 昆明的别称是什么| dha中文叫什么| 颈动脉斑块看什么科| 1月8日是什么星座| 双亲是什么意思| 双侧乳腺小叶增生是什么意思| 冠状动脉粥样硬化性心脏病吃什么药| 拉肚子挂什么科室| 全可以加什么偏旁| 外公的妹妹叫什么| 幸灾乐祸是什么意思| 补肝血吃什么食物最好| 兴风作浪什么意思| 理性什么意思| 金色配什么颜色好看| 右胳膊麻木是什么征兆| 女生肚脐眼下面疼是什么原因| 身上痒什么原因| 5月10日什么星座| 菽是什么意思| 早上起床口臭是什么原因| lala是什么意思| hbsag是什么意思| 益母草长什么样子图片| 什么地找| 梦见雪是什么征兆| 充盈是什么意思| 又字五行属什么| 胆囊切除有什么影响| 频发室性早搏吃什么药| 读书有什么好处| 天仙配是什么剧种| 红楼梦大结局是什么| 凤凰单枞是什么茶| 乘字五行属什么| 一什么枝条| 女性尿检能查出什么病| 站着说话不腰疼是什么意思| 不适是什么意思| 做什么事要从头来| kitty什么意思| 九转大肠是什么菜系| 金鸡独立什么意思| 昭和是什么意思| 大殓是什么意思| 电波系是什么意思| 梅核气吃什么药| ofd是什么意思| 孕妇做唐筛是检查什么| golden是什么牌子| 外阴瘙痒用什么药| 决堤什么意思| 妊娠是什么| 青瓜是什么| 着凉吃什么药| 乳腺钼靶是什么意思| 5月16日是什么星座| 大便有血是什么原因男性| 三文鱼为什么可以生吃| 肌酐500多属于什么期怎么治疗| 闰月鞋买什么颜色| 什么是房补| 灵芝孢子粉有什么用| prp是什么| 瀹是什么意思| 乳酸菌可以制作什么| php是什么意思| 孕初期有什么症状| 杨梅什么时候上市| 宋朝之后是什么朝代| 什么可以代替润滑油| 瑜伽是什么运动| 小鱼际发红预示着什么| 榴莲不可以和什么食物一起吃| 84消毒液不能和什么一起用| 离职什么意思| 女人血稠吃什么食物好| 骶管囊肿是什么意思| 为什么这么热| 农历六月初四是什么日子| 几朵花代表什么意思| 一直不来月经是什么原因| 尿道炎是什么症状| 境遇是什么意思| 狗狗假孕是什么症状| 右附件区囊肿是什么意思| 梦到僵尸是什么预兆| 窝沟封闭是什么意思| 儿保科主要是检查什么| 黄雀是什么鸟| 劣迹斑斑是什么意思| 凌寒独自开的凌是什么意思| 男性尿道疼痛小便刺痛吃什么药| 五谷杂粮是什么| 6.5是什么星座| 狗尾巴草的花语是什么| 土鳖虫吃什么| 鹰潭有什么好玩的地方| 什么是企业年金| 热疹用什么药膏最好| bonnie是什么意思| 夏天吃什么蔬菜好| 蛋白高是什么原因| 医学ac是什么意思| 过期的牛奶有什么用| 商鞅变法是什么时期| 姜字五行属什么| 形体是什么意思| 黄体回声是什么意思| boy是什么牌子| 怀孕初期分泌物是什么样的| 升阳是什么意思| 淋巴结清扫是什么意思| 农历2月12日是什么星座| rhubarb是什么意思| 什么是子宫腺肌症| 海棠依旧什么意思| 7月29号是什么星座| 马蹄铁什么时候发明的| 什么是前庭功能| 神经损伤吃什么药| 247什么意思| 后果自负是什么意思| 维生素d什么时候吃| 什么地指挥| 资治通鉴讲的是什么| 红玫瑰花语是什么意思| 海绵是什么材料做的| 旧衣服属于什么垃圾| 雷锋代表什么生肖| 立春之后是什么节气| 儿童吃什么钙片补钙效果好| 支气管哮喘吃什么药| pt是什么单位| 为什么冬天容易长胖| 非洲是什么人种| 亥是什么意思| diptyque属于什么档次| 胎停有什么症状| 间皮瘤是什么病| 百度Jump to content

TFBOYS王源打球啥水平? 旋转跳跃篮板后神奇投篮

From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia
百度 肖全只是一个快乐影像的采集者,是一个美好故事的收集人。

The spacing effect demonstrates that learning is more effective when study sessions are spaced out. This effect shows that more information is encoded into long-term memory by spaced study sessions, also known as spaced repetition or spaced presentation, than by massed presentation ("cramming").

The phenomenon was first identified by Hermann Ebbinghaus, and his detailed study of it was published in the 1885 book über das Ged?chtnis. Untersuchungen zur experimentellen Psychologie (Memory: A Contribution to Experimental Psychology), which suggests that active recall with increasing time intervals reduces the probability of forgetting information. This robust finding has been supported by studies of many explicit memory tasks such as free recall, recognition, cued-recall, and frequency estimation (for reviews see Crowder 1976; Greene, 1989).

Researchers have offered several possible explanations of the spacing effect, and much research has been conducted that supports its impact on recall. In spite of these findings, the robustness of this phenomenon and its resistance to experimental manipulation have made empirical testing of its parameters difficult.

While many others have contributed important research regarding the spacing effect, Robert Bjork and his associates in the Bjork Learning and Forgetting Lab and Cogfog group at UCLA have performed much research into various aspects of this phenomenon as well as into its practical application for education.[1]

Causes

[edit]

Decades of research on memory and recall have produced many different theories and findings on the spacing effect. In a study conducted by Cepeda et al. (2006) participants who used spaced practice on memory tasks outperformed those using massed practice in 259 out of 271 cases.

As different studies support different aspects of this effect, some now believe that an appropriate account should be multi-factorial, and at present, different mechanisms are invoked to account for the spacing effect in free recall and in cued-memory tasks.

Not much attention has been given to the study of the spacing effect in long-term retention tests. Shaughnessy (1977) found that the spacing effect is not robust for items presented twice after a 24-hour delay in testing. The spacing effect is present, however, for items presented four or six times and tested after a 24-hour delay. The result was interesting because other studies using only twice-presented items have shown a strong spacing effect, although the lag between learning and testing was longer. Shaughnessy interprets it as evidence that no single explanatory mechanism can be used to account for the various manifestations of the spacing effect.[2]

Semantic priming

[edit]

Research has shown reliable spacing effects in cued-memory tasks under incidental learning conditions, where semantic analysis is encouraged through orienting tasks (Challis, 1993; Russo & Mammaralla, 2002). Challis found a spacing effect for target words using a frequency estimation task after words were incidentally analyzed semantically. However, no spacing effect was found when the target words were shallowly encoded using a graphemic study task. This suggests that semantic priming underlies the spacing effect in cued-memory tasks. When items are presented in a massed fashion, the first occurrence of the target semantically primes the mental representation of that target, such that when the second occurrence appears directly after the first, there is a reduction in its semantic processing. Semantic priming wears off after a period of time (Kirsner, Smith, Lockhart, & King, 1984), which is why there is less semantic priming of the second occurrence of a spaced item. Thus on the semantic priming account, the second presentation is more strongly primed and receives less semantic processing when the repetitions are massed compared to when presentations are spaced over short lags (Challis, 1993). This semantic priming mechanism provides spaced words with more extensive processing than massed words, producing the spacing effect.

From this explanation of the spacing effect, it follows that this effect should not occur with nonsense stimuli that do not have a semantic representation in memory. A number of studies have demonstrated that the semantically based repetition priming approach cannot explain spacing effects in recognition memory for stimuli such as unfamiliar faces, and non-words that are not amenable to semantic analysis (Russo, Parkin, Taylor, & Wilks, 1998; Russo et al., 2002; Mammarella, Russo, & Avons, 2005). Cornoldi and Longoni (1977) have even found a significant spacing effect in a forced-choice recognition memory task when nonsense shapes were used as target stimuli. Russo et al. (1998) proposed that with cued memory of unfamiliar stimuli, a short-term perceptually-based repetition priming mechanism supports the spacing effect. When unfamiliar stimuli are used as targets in a cued-memory task, memory relies on the retrieval of structural-perceptual information about the targets. When the items are presented in a massed fashion, the first occurrence primes its second occurrence, leading to reduced perceptual processing of the second presentation. Short-term repetition-priming effects for nonwords are reduced when the lag between prime and target trials is reduced from zero to six (McKone, 1995), thus it follows that more extensive perceptual processing is given to the second occurrence of spaced items relative to that given to massed items. Hence, nonsense items with massed presentation receive less extensive perceptual processing than spaced items; thus, the retrieval of those items is impaired in cued-memory tasks.

Congruent with this view, Russo et al. (2002) demonstrated that changing the font in which repeated presentations of nonwords were presented reduced the short-term perceptual priming of those stimuli, especially for massed items. Upon a recognition memory test, there was no spacing effect found for the nonwords presented in different fonts during study. These results support the hypothesis that short-term perceptual priming is the mechanism that supports the spacing effects in cued-memory tasks when unfamiliar stimuli are used as targets. Furthermore, when the font was changed between repeated presentations of words in the study phase, there was no reduction of the spacing effect. This resistance to the font manipulation is expected with this two-factor account, as semantic processing of words at study determines performance on a later memory test, and the font manipulation is irrelevant to this form of processing.

Mammarella, Russo, & Avons (2002) also demonstrated that changing the orientation of faces between repeated presentations served to eliminate the spacing effect. Unfamiliar faces do not have stored representations in memory, thus the spacing effect for these stimuli would be a result of perceptual priming. Changing orientation served to alter the physical appearance of the stimuli, thus reducing the perceptual priming at the second occurrence of the face when presented in a massed fashion. This led to equal memory for faces presented in massed and spaced fashions, hence eliminating the spacing effect.

Encoding variability

[edit]

The encoding variability theory holds that performance on a memory test is determined by the overlap between the available contextual information during the test and the contextual information available during the encoding.[3] According to this view, spaced repetition typically entails some variability in presentation contexts, resulting in a greater number of retrieval cues. Contrastingly, massed repetitions have limited presentations and therefore fewer retrieval cues. The notion of the efficacy of the increased variability of encoding is supported by the position that the more independent encodings are, the more different types of cues are associated with an item.[4]

There are two types of encoding variability theory that address the spacing effect. The first maintains that the spacing effect refers to the changes in the semantic interpretations of items which cause the effect, while the second holds that variability surrounding context is responsible for the spacing effect, not only semantic variability.[4]

To test the encoding variability theory, Bird, Nicholson, and Ringer (1978)[5] presented subjects with word lists that either had massed or spaced repetitions. Subjects were asked to perform various "orienting tasks", tasks which require the subject to make a simple judgment about the list item (i.e. pleasant or unpleasant, active or passive). Subjects either performed the same task for each occurrence of a word or a different task for each occurrence. If the encoding variability theory were true, then the case of different orienting tasks ought to provide variable encoding, even for massed repetitions, resulting in a higher rate of recall for massed repetitions than would be expected. The results showed no such effect, providing strong evidence against the importance of encoding variability.

Study-phase retrieval theory

[edit]

A theory that has gained a lot of traction recently[when?] is the study-phase retrieval theory. This theory assumes that the first presentation is retrieved at the time of the second. This leads to an elaboration of the first memory trace. Massed presentations do not yield advantages because the first trace is active at the time of the second, so it is not retrieved or elaborated upon. Greene (1989) proposed a two-factor account of the spacing effect, combining deficient processing and study-phase retrieval accounts. Spacing effects in free recall are accounted for by the study-phase retrieval account. Under the assumption that free recall is sensitive to contextual associations, spaced items are at an advantage over massed items by the additional encoding of contextual information. Thus, the second occurrence of an item in a list reminds the observer of the previous occurrence of that same item and of its previous contextual features. Different contextual information is encoded with each presentation, whereas for massed items, the difference in context is relatively small. More retrieval cues, then, are encoded with spaced learning, which in turn leads to improved recall.

Deficient processing

[edit]

According to the deficient processing view, massed repetitions lead to deficient processing of the second presentation—that we simply do not pay much attention to the later presentations (Hintzman et al., 1973). Greene (1989) proposed this to be the case in cued-memory tasks (e.g. recognition memory, frequency estimation tasks), which rely more on item information and less on contextual information. The increased voluntary rehearsal of spaced items makes this deficient processing noticeable. Findings that the spacing effect is not found when items are studied through incidental learning support this account.

Retrieval effort hypothesis

[edit]

According to research conducted by Pyc and Rawson (2009), successful but effortful retrieval tasks during practice enhance memory in an account known as the retrieval effort hypothesis. Spacing out the learning and relearning of items leads to a more effortful retrieval which provides for deeper processing of the item.

Practical applications and long-term retention

[edit]

Advertising

[edit]

The spacing effect and its underlying mechanisms have important applications to the world of advertising. For instance, the spacing effect dictates that it is not an effective advertising strategy to present the same commercial back-to-back (massed repetition). Spaced ads were remembered better than ads that had been repeated back to back.[6] Layout variations presented in short spacing intervals also resulted in improved recall compared to ads presented in exact repetition.[6] The same effect was also achieved in a study involving website advertisements. It was revealed that sales diminish progressively as the customer visited the site and was exposed to the ad several times. However, if the elapsed time between the visits was longer, the advertisement had a bigger effect on sales.[7] If encoding variability is an important mechanism of the spacing effect, then a good advertising strategy might include a distributed presentation of different versions of the same ad.

Appleton-Knapp, Bjork and Wickens (2005)[8] examined the effects of spacing on advertising. They found that spaced repetitions of advertisements are more affected by study-phase retrieval processes than encoding variability. They also found that at long intervals, varying the presentation of a given ad is not effective in producing higher recall rates among subjects (as predicted by variable encoding). Despite this finding, recognition is not affected by variations in an ad at long intervals.

Application in education

[edit]

Studies have shown that long-term spacing effects are prevalent in learning and produce significant learning gains, particularly when the spacing gaps are on the order of days or weeks.[9] Although it is accepted that spacing is beneficial in learning a subject well and previous units should be revisited and practiced, textbooks are written in discrete chapters that do not support these findings. Rohrer conducted a two-part study in 2006 where students were taught how to solve math problems.[10] In part 1, students either used mass or spaced practice, and spaced practice showed significant improvement over mass practice when tested one week later. In the second part of the experiment, practice problems were either grouped by type or mixed randomly. The desirable difficulties encountered by the randomly mixed problems were effective, and the performance by students who solved the randomly mixed problems was vastly superior to the students who solved the problems grouped by type. The reasoning behind this increased performance was that students know the formula for solving equations, but do not always know when to apply the formula. By shuffling problems around and dispersing them across multiple chapters, students also learn to identify when it is appropriate to use which formula. There is conclusive evidence that cumulative final exams promote long-term retention by forcing spaced learning to occur.

Learning and pedagogy

[edit]

The long-term effects of spacing have also been assessed in the context of learning a foreign language. Bahrick et al. (1993)[11] examined the retention of newly learned foreign vocabulary words as a function of relearning sessions and intersession spacing over a nine-year period.

Both the amount of relearning sessions and the number of days in between each session have a major impact on retention (the repetition effect and the spacing effect), yet the two variables do not interact with each other.

For all three difficulty rankings of the foreign words, recall was highest for the 56-day interval as opposed to a 28-day or a 14-day interval. Additionally, 13 sessions spaced 56 days apart yielded comparable retention to 26 sessions with a 14-day interval.

These findings have implications for educational practices. Current school and university curricula rarely provide students with opportunities for periodic retrieval of previously acquired knowledge.[12] Without spaced repetitions, students are more likely to forget foreign language vocabulary.

Lag effect

[edit]

While the spacing effect refers to improved recall for spaced versus successive (mass) repetition, the term 'lag' can be interpreted as the time interval between repetitions of learning. The lag effect is simply an idea branching off the spacing effect that states recall after long lags between learning is better versus short lags.[13] Michael Kahana's study showed strong evidence that the lag effect is present when recalling word lists. In 2008, Kornell and Bjork published a study[14] that suggested inductive learning is more effective when spaced than massed. Inductive learning is learning through observation of exemplars, so the participants did not actively take notes or solve problems. These results were replicated and backed up by a second independent study.[15]

See also

[edit]

References

[edit]
  1. ^ "Research | Bjork Learning and Forgetting Lab". bjorklab.psych.ucla.edu. Retrieved February 25, 2024.
  2. ^ Shaughnessy, John J. (1977). "Long-Term Retention and the Spacing Effect in Free-Recall and Frequency Judgments". The American Journal of Psychology. 90 (4): 587–598. doi:10.2307/1421733. ISSN 0002-9556. JSTOR 1421733.
  3. ^ Nairne, James (2007). The Foundations of Remembering: Essays in Honor of Henry L. Roediger, III. New York: Psychology Press. p. 85. ISBN 9781841694467.
  4. ^ a b Cormier, S. M. (April 4, 2014). Basic Processes of Learning, Cognition, and Motivation. Psychology Press. ISBN 9781317757481.
  5. ^ Bird, Charles P.; Nicholson, Angus J.; Ringer, Susan (1978). "Resistance of the Spacing Effect to Variations in Encoding". The American Journal of Psychology. 91 (4): 713–721. doi:10.2307/1421519. ISSN 0002-9556. JSTOR 1421519.
  6. ^ a b Fennis, Bob; Stroebe, Wolfgang (2010). The Psychology of Advertising. Hove: Psychology Press. p. 104. ISBN 978-0203853238.
  7. ^ Wierenga, Berend; Lans, Ralf van der (2017). Handbook of Marketing Decision Models, Second Edition. Cham: Springer. p. 193. ISBN 9783319569390.
  8. ^ Appleton-Knapp, Sara L. (2005). "Examining the Spacing Effect in Advertising: Encoding Variability, Retrieval Processes, and Their Interaction". Journal of Consumer Research. 32 (2): 266–276. doi:10.1086/432236.
  9. ^ Byrne, John (2017). Learning and Memory: A Comprehensive Reference. Boston, MA: Academic Press. p. 481. ISBN 9780128051597.
  10. ^ Rohrer, Doug; Taylor, Kelli (April 19, 2007). "The shuffling of mathematics problems improves learning". Instructional Science. 35 (6): 481–498. doi:10.1007/s11251-007-9015-8. S2CID 55686289. Retrieved April 23, 2016.
  11. ^ Bahrick, Harry P.; Bahrick, Lorraine E.; Bahrick, Audrey S.; Bahrick, Phyllis E. (1993). "Maintenance of Foreign Language Vocabulary and the Spacing Effect". Psychological Science. 4 (5): 316–321. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.1993.tb00571.x. ISSN 0956-7976. JSTOR 40063054.
  12. ^ "The death of the university lecture", Huffington Post, retrieved 2025-08-14
  13. ^ Kahana, Michael (2005). "Spacing and lag effects in free recall of pure lists" (PDF). Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. 12 (1): 159–164. doi:10.3758/bf03196362. PMID 15948289. S2CID 18549788. Retrieved April 22, 2016.
  14. ^ Kornell, Bjork, Nate, Robert (2008). "Is Spacing the "Enemy of Induction"?" (PDF). Psychological Science. 19 (6): 585–592. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02127.x. PMID 18578849. S2CID 18139036. Retrieved April 23, 2016.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  15. ^ Verkoeijen, Peter P. J. L.; Bouwmeester, Samantha (January 1, 2014). "Is spacing really the "friend of induction"?". Frontiers in Psychology. 5: 259. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00259. ISSN 1664-1078. PMC 3978334. PMID 24744742.
  • Appleton-Knapp, S.L.; Bjork, R.A.; Wickens, T.D. (2005). "Examining the spacing effect in advertising: Encoding variability, retrieval processes, and their interaction". Journal of Consumer Research. 32 (2): 266–276. doi:10.1086/432236.
  • Bird, C.P. (1987). "Influence of the spacing of trait information on impressions of likability". Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. 23 (6): 481–497. doi:10.1016/0022-1031(87)90017-5.
  • Cepeda, N. J.; Pashler, H.; Vul, E.; Wixted, J. T.; Rohrer, D. (2006). "Distributed practice in verbal recall tasks: A review and quantitative synthesis". Psychological Bulletin. 132 (3): 354–380. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.408.6627. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.132.3.354. PMID 16719566. S2CID 18831615.
  • Cermak, L.S.; Verfaellie, M.; Lanzoni, S.; Mather, M.; Chase, K.A. (1996). "Effect of spaced repetitions on amnesia patients' recall and recognition performance". Neuropsychology. 10 (2): 219–227. doi:10.1037/0894-4105.10.2.219.
  • Challis, B.H. (1993). "Spacing effects on cued-memory tests depend on level of processing". Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition. 19 (2): 389–396. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.19.2.389.
  • Crowder, R.G. (1976). Principles of learning and memory. Oxford, England: Lawrence Erlbaum.
  • Dempster, F.N. (1988). "Informing classroom practice: What we know about several task characteristics and their effects on learning". Contemporary Educational Psychology. 13 (3): 254–264. doi:10.1016/0361-476x(88)90025-2.
  • Dempster, F.N. (1988). "The spacing effect: A case study in the failure to apply the results of psychological research". American Psychologist. 43 (8): 627–634. doi:10.1037/0003-066x.43.8.627. S2CID 6644335.
  • Ebbinghaus, Hermann (1885). über das Ged?chtnis. Untersuchungen zur experimentellen Psychologie [Memory: A Contribution to Experimental Psychology] (in German). Trans. Henry A. Ruger & Clara E. Bussenius. Leipzig, Germany: Duncker & Humblot.
  • Greene R. L. (2008). Repetition and spacing effects. In Roediger H. L. III (Ed.), Learning and memory: A comprehensive reference. Cognitive Psychology of Memory, 2, 65–78. Oxford: Elsevier.
  • Greene, R.L. (1989). "Spacing effects in memory: Evidence for a two-process account". Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition. 15 (3): 371–377. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.15.3.371.
  • Hintzman, D.L. (1974). Theoretical implications of the spacing effect. Theories in Cognitive Psychology: The Loyola Symposium. Oxford, England: Lawrence Erlbaum.
  • Leicht, K.L.; Overton, R. (1987). "Encoding variability and spacing repetitions". American Journal of Psychology. 100 (1): 61–68. doi:10.2307/1422642. JSTOR 1422642.
  • Mammarella, N.; Avons, S.E.; Russo, R. (2004). "A short-term perceptual priming account of spacing effects in explicit cued-memory tasks for unfamiliar stimuli". European Journal of Cognitive Psychology. 16 (3): 387–402. doi:10.1080/09541440340000042. S2CID 144292880.
  • Mammarella, N.; Russo, R.; Avons, S.E. (2002). "Spacing effects in cued-memory tasks for unfamiliar faces and nonwords". Memory & Cognition. 30 (8): 1238–1251. doi:10.3758/bf03213406. PMID 12661855.
  • Pyc, M. A.; Rawson, K. A. (2009). "Testing retrieval efforts hypothesis: Does greater difficulty correctly recalling information lead to higher levels of memory?". Journal of Memory and Language. 60 (4): 437–447. doi:10.1016/j.jml.2009.01.004.
  • Rawson, K. A.; Dunlosky, J. (2012). "Relearning Attenuates the Benefits and Costs of Spacing". Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. 142 (4): 1113–1129. doi:10.1037/a0030498. PMID 23088488.
  • Russo, R.; Ma; Wilks, J. (1998). "Revising current two-process accounts of spacing effects in memory". Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition. 24 (1): 161–172. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.24.1.161. PMID 9438957.
  • Toppino, T.C.; Bloom, L.C. (2002). "The spacing effect, free recall, and two-process theory: A closer look". Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition. 28 (3): 437–444. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.387.5552. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.28.3.437. PMID 12018496.
  • Whitten, W.B.; Bjork, R.A. (1977). "Learning from tests: Effects of spacing". Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior. 16 (4): 465–478. doi:10.1016/s0022-5371(77)80040-6.
  • Wozniak, P.A.; Gorzelanczyk, E.J. (1994). "Optimization of repetition spacing in the practice of learning". Acta Neurobiologiae Experimentalis. 54 (1): 59–62. doi:10.55782/ane-1994-1003. PMID 8023714.
  • Young, D.R.; Bellezza, F.S. (1982). "Encoding variability, memory organization, and the repetition effect". Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition. 8 (6): 545–559. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.8.6.545.
[edit]
鸡骨草有什么功效 海啸是什么 屎是什么味道的 前方高能什么意思 蟾蜍属于什么动物
万里长城是什么生肖 忍者神龟是什么意思 保妇康栓是治疗什么的 男生为什么会勃起 一九八三年属什么生肖
脾大是什么原因引起的 姜黄与生姜有什么区别 女子与小人难养也什么意思 多囊卵巢综合征吃什么药 喝酒吃海带有什么危害
姜子牙属什么生肖 动物园有什么动物 鳞状上皮增生什么意思 鼻子经常流鼻涕是什么原因 梦见自己生个女孩是什么意思
什么是处方药clwhiglsz.com 嘴角疱疹用什么药膏shenchushe.com 德国纳粹是什么意思liaochangning.com g50是什么高速hcv8jop7ns3r.cn 弊端是什么意思hcv8jop0ns3r.cn
梦到水是什么意思hcv8jop4ns5r.cn 人为什么会变hcv9jop1ns3r.cn 为什么水不能燃烧gysmod.com 杀阴虱用什么药最好hcv8jop0ns5r.cn 羊排炖什么好吃hcv8jop3ns7r.cn
女人怀孕的最佳时间是什么时间hanqikai.com 儿童包皮挂什么科hcv8jop4ns4r.cn 格林是什么意思hcv8jop6ns4r.cn 慢性荨麻疹是什么症状hcv7jop4ns5r.cn 焕字五行属什么hcv8jop6ns0r.cn
什么病能办低保hcv8jop4ns8r.cn 诸神黄昏什么意思hcv9jop2ns5r.cn 妃子是什么意思hcv9jop7ns0r.cn 牙齿一吸就出血是什么原因hcv8jop7ns9r.cn 哈伦裤配什么上衣好看hcv9jop4ns2r.cn
百度