hbv是什么意思| 两岁宝宝不会说话但什么都知道| 心脏吃什么药最好| 健康证是什么样的| 五月二十三日是什么星座| 黯然泪下是什么意思| 什么是真爱| 跑步对身体有什么好处| 为什么要做包皮手术| 鼻子痒用什么药| fsh是什么| 电子证件照是什么| 嗜碱性粒细胞偏高是什么原因| 备孕需要检查什么| 喝柠檬水对身体有什么好处| 擦什么能阻止毛发生长| 黄花是什么花| 米索前列醇片是什么药| 脸上黑色的小点是什么| b型钠尿肽高说明什么| 康庄大道是什么意思| 开拔是什么意思| 伊朗用什么语言| 耳鸣和脑鸣有什么区别| 美甲光疗是什么| 44岁月经量少是什么原因| 衣原体是什么| 鸡精吃多了有什么危害| 睡着了流口水是什么原因| 伸张正义是什么意思| 炖鸡块放什么调料| 狐狸吊坠有什么寓意| 辩证法是什么| 疫情是什么| 什么是一桌餐| 月经第二天属于什么期| 脖子肿是什么原因| 最好的补钙方法是什么| 眉头下方有痣代表什么| 谷子是什么意思| 脚筋疼是什么原因| 天线宝宝都叫什么名字| 下面潮湿是什么原因引起的| 羟氯喹是什么药| 女性更年期潮热出汗吃什么药| 嘴唇舌头发麻什么病兆| 产后抑郁症有什么表现症状| 乳腺结节三级是什么意思| 神经衰弱吃什么药最好| 涵字属于五行属什么| 人为什么| 包皮炎吃什么药| 盆腔磁共振平扫能查出什么| 很什么很什么| 娇韵诗属于什么档次| 后厨是做什么的| 驼背挂什么科| 专员是什么级别| 灰指甲是什么样子| 舌苔黑是什么病| 滴虫性阴炎有什么症状表现| 小肠气是什么症状| 豆芽炒什么好吃| 为什么眼睛会痛| 在眼皮老跳是什么征兆| 一例是什么意思| 女人肺气虚吃什么补最快| 鼻子上长痘痘是什么原因| 什么的马| 摩托车代表什么生肖| 一九三七年属什么生肖| 什么化妆品好用| 猫代表什么数字| 鬼节为什么不能出去| 湄公鱼是什么鱼| 铁面无私是什么生肖| 女人吃什么补充雌激素| 梦到大牙掉了一颗是什么意思| 什么药退烧快| 迟缓是什么意思| 相恋纪念日送什么礼物| 糖尿病人吃什么主食| 孩子脾虚内热大便干吃什么药| 什么照片看不出照的是谁| 心肌桥是什么意思| 跳蚤喜欢咬什么样的人| 小猫呕吐吃什么药| 生化流产是什么原因造成的| 腰痛吃什么好| 南瓜不能和什么一起吃| pt什么意思| 小鹿乱撞是什么意思| 梦见好多猫是什么意思| 血压高为什么| 崩漏下血是什么意思| 木薯是什么| 一暴十寒什么意思| 脚气是什么菌引起的| 大米放什么不生虫子| 回民是什么意思| 树菠萝什么时候成熟| 单从属于什么茶| 辛苦是什么意思| 仰面朝天是什么生肖| as材质是什么材料| 九月二十六是什么星座| 两仪是什么意思| 一什么一什么词语| 什么样的晚霞| 挑疳积挑出来的是什么| 眼皮浮肿是什么原因| 乳臭未干是什么意思| 梦到打死蛇是什么意思| 文定之喜是什么意思| 舌头裂纹是什么病| 身上没长什么就是干痒| 京东公司全称是什么| 孕酮代表什么| 日后好相见的前一句是什么| 对牛弹琴代表什么生肖| 双鱼座的幸运色是什么颜色| 金渐层是什么品种| 参考是什么意思| 什么叫刺身| 吃了龙虾后不能吃什么| mom是什么意思| 脚后跟麻木是什么原因| 打三个喷嚏代表什么| 为什么家里会有蟑螂| 辣椒有什么营养价值| 喝什么胸会变大| 7月初7是什么日子| 蛋白质用什么试剂鉴定| crs是什么意思| 轴向是什么意思| 大生化挂什么科| 在什么地方| 美国全称是什么| 糜烂性胃炎有什么症状| 为什么会莫名其妙的哭| 妃是什么意思| 寓教于乐什么意思| 一笑泯恩仇什么意思| 乾元是什么意思| 吃什么水果能美白| 腰肌劳损看什么科| 容易脸红的人是什么原因| 张学良为什么被囚禁| 奇妙是什么意思| 清明节吃什么| 开放性骨折是什么意思| 肝阳上亢吃什么中成药| 坐飞机不能带什么物品| 解脲脲原体阳性吃什么药| 胎盘做成胶囊吃有什么好处| 天壤之别是什么意思| 仓鼠用什么洗澡| 肌酐是什么意思| 钙化结节是什么意思| 泌尿科看什么病| 中气下陷是什么意思| 一个口一个甫念什么| 食管反流用什么药效果好| 刘晓庆什么星座| 什么的口罩| 送礼物送什么好| 菠菜和什么不能一起吃| 周杰伦什么病| 一加一为什么等于二| 恭请是什么意思| 糜烂性胃炎有什么症状| 包皮炎看什么科| 尿液中有血是什么原因| 什么是亚健康| 气短是什么症状| 什么东西清肺止咳| 澳门用什么币种| 照见五蕴皆空什么意思| 血液透析是什么意思| 生蛇是什么病| 小孩反复发烧是什么原因引起的| 女性缓解疲劳吃什么好| 发绀是什么意思| 冠字五行属什么| 18年是什么婚| 吃羊肉不能吃什么| 为什么脚会肿起来是什么原因| 什么是绘本| 鉴黄师是什么职业| 水溶性是什么意思| 紧急避孕药什么时候吃有效| 什么情况下需要割包皮| 腹膜转移是什么意思| 人体是由什么组成的| 肚子疼吃什么食物好| 今年什么时间进伏| hcg值低是什么原因| 经常拉屎是什么原因| 小暑是什么意思啊| 迪卡侬属于什么档次| 毛戈平化妆品什么档次| 一命呜呼是什么意思| b超和阴超有什么区别| 结核抗体弱阳性什么意思| 什么是芝士| 生花生吃了有什么好处| 什么的蜡烛| 中国最长的河流是什么河| 八三年属什么生肖| 嗜血综合症是什么病| 阴道有味道是什么原因| 皮炎吃什么药| 小腿怕冷是什么原因| 舌头痛吃什么药好| 马头琴是什么族的乐器| 桑叶泡水喝有什么功效和作用| 负荷是什么意思| 2月6号是什么星座| 梅雨季节什么时候结束| 你的美丽让你带走是什么歌| 牙齿突然酸痛什么原因| 眼睛出现重影是什么原因| 为什么会莫名其妙的哭| 离子四项是检查什么的| 怨妇是什么意思| 蜘蛛吃什么| 盯眝是什么意思| 两棵树是什么牌子| 92年是什么命| 孩子经常发烧是什么原因| 皮肤长斑是什么原因引起的| 母亲节送婆婆什么礼物| 什么是中药| 胃疼吃什么药最管用| 尿潜血弱阳性是什么意思| 什么是多囊卵巢综合症| 两棵树是什么牌子| 刘的五行属什么| 留个念想是什么意思| 拉肚子引起的发烧吃什么药| 山楂泡酒有什么功效| 男生来大姨夫是什么意思| 赤道2什么时候上映| 党参长什么样图片| 硬度不够是什么原因| 难缠是什么意思| 三什么五什么| 为什么有的人皮肤黑| 虚不受补吃什么中成药| 月经期间适合吃什么| 待客是什么意思| 咳嗽吃什么水果| 杰瑞是什么品种的老鼠| 因缘际会是什么意思| 人工受孕和试管婴儿有什么区别| 2037年是什么年| 问是什么结构| 落井下石是什么意思| 左下腹疼痛挂什么科| 间接胆红素偏高吃什么药| 什么是电解质水| 共济会是什么| 什么牙膏最好| 左下眼皮跳是什么预兆| 百度Jump to content

尿微肌酐比值高是什么情况

Listen to this article
From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia

A vowel is a speech sound pronounced without any stricture in the vocal tract,[1] forming the nucleus of a syllable. Vowels are one of the two principal classes of speech sounds, the other being the consonant. Vowels vary in quality, in loudness and also in quantity (length). They are usually voiced and are closely involved in prosodic variation such as tone, intonation and stress.

The word vowel comes from the Latin word vocalis, meaning "vocal" (i.e. relating to the voice).[2] In English, the word vowel is commonly used to refer both to vowel sounds and to the written symbols that represent them (⟨a⟩, ⟨e⟩, ⟨i⟩, ⟨o⟩, ⟨u⟩, and sometimes ⟨w⟩ and ⟨y⟩).[3]

An idealized schematic of vowel space, based on the formants of Daniel Jones and John Wells pronouncing the cardinal vowels of the IPA. The scale is logarithmic. The grey range is where F2 would be less than F1, which by definition is impossible. [a] is an extra-low central vowel. Phonemically it may be front or back, depending on the language. Rounded vowels that are front in tongue position are front-central in formant space, while unrounded vowels that are back in articulation are back-central in formant space. Thus [y ?] have perhaps similar F1 and F2 values to the high central vowels [? ?], being distinguished by rounding (F3); similarly [? ?] vs central [? ?] and [? ?] vs central [? ?].

Definition

[edit]

There are two complementary definitions of vowel, one phonetic and the other phonological.

  • In the phonetic definition, a vowel is a sound, such as the English "ah" /ɑ?/ or "oh" /o?/, produced with an open vocal tract; it is median (the air escapes along the middle of the tongue), oral (at least some of the airflow must escape through the mouth), frictionless and continuant.[4] There is no significant build-up of air pressure at any point above the glottis. This contrasts with consonants, such as the English "sh" [?], which have a constriction or closure at some point along the vocal tract.
  • In the phonological definition, a vowel is defined as syllabic, the sound that forms the peak of a syllable.[5] A phonetically equivalent but non-syllabic sound is a semivowel. In oral languages, phonetic vowels normally form the peak (nucleus) of many or all syllables, whereas consonants form the onset and (in languages that have them) coda. Some languages allow other sounds to form the nucleus of a syllable, such as the syllabic (i.e., vocalic) l in the English word table [?t?e?.bl?] (when not considered to have a weak vowel sound: [?t?e?.b?l]) or the syllabic r in the Serbo-Croatian word vrt [?r??t] "garden".

The phonetic definition of "vowel" (i.e. a sound produced with no constriction in the vocal tract) does not always match the phonological definition (i.e. a sound that forms the peak of a syllable).[6] The approximants [j] and [w] illustrate this: both are without much of a constriction in the vocal tract (so phonetically they seem to be vowel-like), but they occur at the onset of syllables (e.g. in "yet" and "wet") which suggests that phonologically they are consonants. A similar debate arises over whether a word like bird in a rhotic dialect has an r-colored vowel /?/ or a syllabic consonant /??/. The American linguist Kenneth Pike (1943) suggested the terms "vocoid" for a phonetic vowel and "vowel" for a phonological vowel,[7] so using this terminology, [j] and [w] are classified as vocoids but not vowels. However, Maddieson and Emmory (1985) demonstrated from a range of languages that semivowels are produced with a narrower constriction of the vocal tract than vowels, and so may be considered consonants on that basis.[8] Nonetheless, the phonetic and phonemic definitions would still conflict for the syllabic /l/ in table or the syllabic nasals in button and rhythm.

Articulation

[edit]
X-rays of Daniel Jones' [i, u, a, ɑ]
The original vowel quadrilateral, from Jones' articulation. The vowel trapezoid of the modern IPA, and at the top of this article, is a simplified rendition of this diagram. The bullets are the cardinal vowel points. (A parallel diagram covers the front and central rounded and back unrounded vowels.) The cells indicate the ranges of articulation that could reasonably be transcribed with those cardinal vowel letters, [i, e, ?, a, ɑ, ?, o, u, ?], and non-cardinal [?]. If a language distinguishes fewer than these vowel qualities, [e, ?] could be merged to ⟨e⟩, [o, ?] to ⟨o⟩, [a, ɑ] to ⟨a⟩, etc. If a language distinguishes more, ⟨?⟩ could be added where the ranges of [i, e, ?, ?] intersect, ⟨?⟩ where [u, o, ?, ?] intersect, and ⟨?⟩ where [?, ?, a, ɑ, ?] intersect.

The traditional view of vowel production, reflected for example in the terminology and presentation of the International Phonetic Alphabet, is one of articulatory features that determine a vowel's quality as distinguishing it from other vowels. Daniel Jones developed the cardinal vowel system to describe vowels in terms of the features of tongue height (vertical dimension), tongue backness (horizontal dimension) and roundedness (lip articulation). These three parameters are indicated in the schematic quadrilateral IPA vowel diagram on the right. There are additional features of vowel quality, such as the velum position (nasality), type of vocal fold vibration (phonation), and tongue root position.

This conception of vowel articulation has been known to be inaccurate since 1928. Peter Ladefoged has said that "early phoneticians... thought they were describing the highest point of the tongue, but they were not. They were actually describing formant frequencies."[9] (See below.) The IPA Handbook concedes that "the vowel quadrilateral must be regarded as an abstraction and not a direct mapping of tongue position."[10]

Nonetheless, the concept that vowel qualities are determined primarily by tongue position and lip rounding continues to be used in pedagogy, as it provides an intuitive explanation of how vowels are distinguished.

Height

[edit]

Theoretically, vowel height refers to the vertical position of either the tongue or the jaw (depending on the model) relative to either the roof of the mouth or the aperture of the jaw. In practice, however, it refers to the first formant (lowest resonance of the voice), abbreviated F1, which is associated with the height of the tongue. There are two terms commonly applied to refer to two degrees of vowel height: in close vowels, also known as high vowels, such as [i] and [u], the first formant is consistent with the tongue being positioned close to the palate, high in the mouth, whereas in open vowels, also known as low vowels, such as [a], F1 is consistent with the jaw being open and the tongue being positioned low in the mouth. Height is defined by the inverse of the F1 value: the higher the frequency of the first formant, the lower (more open) the vowel.[a] In John Esling's usage, where fronted vowels are distinguished in height by the position of the jaw rather than the tongue, only the terms 'open' and 'close' are used, as 'high' and 'low' refer to the position of the tongue.

The International Phonetic Alphabet has letters for six degrees of vowel height for full vowels (plus the reduced mid vowel [?]), but it is extremely unusual for a language to distinguish this many degrees without other attributes. The IPA letters distinguish (sorted according to height, with the top-most one being the highest and the bottom-most being the lowest):

The letters ⟨e, ?, ?, ?, ?, o⟩ are defined as close-mid but are commonly used for true mid vowels. If more precision is required, true mid vowels may be written with a lowering or raising diacritic: ⟨e?, ??, ??, ??, ??, o?⟩ or ⟨?? ?? ?? ?? ?? ??⟩.

The Kensiu language, spoken in Malaysia and Thailand, is highly unusual in contrasting true mid vowels with both close-mid and open-mid vowels, without any additional parameters such as length, roundness or ATR. The front vowels, /i ? e e? ?/, along with open /a/, make a six-way height distinction; this holds even for the nasal vowels. A few varieties of German have been reported to have five contrastive vowel heights that are independent of length or other parameters. For example, the Bavarian dialect of Amstetten has thirteen long vowels, which have been analyzed as four vowel heights (close, close-mid, mid, open-mid) each among the front unrounded, front rounded, and back rounded vowels, along with an open vowel for a fifth height: /i e ?? ?/, /y ? ?? ?/, /u o ?? ?/, /a/. Apart from the aforementioned Kensiu language, no other language is known to contrast more than four degrees of vowel height.

The parameter of vowel height appears to be the primary cross-linguistic feature of vowels in that all spoken languages that have been researched till now use height as a contrastive feature. No other parameter, even backness or rounding (see below), is used in all languages. Some languages have vertical vowel systems in which at least at a phonemic level, only height is used to distinguish vowels.

Backness

[edit]
Idealistic tongue positions of cardinal front vowels with highest point indicated

Vowel backness is named for the position of the tongue during the articulation of a vowel relative to the back of the mouth. As with vowel height, however, it is defined by a formant of the voice, in this case the second, F2, not by the position of the tongue. In front vowels, such as [i], the frequency of F2 is relatively high, which generally corresponds to a position of the tongue forward in the mouth, whereas in back vowels, such as [u], F2 is low, consistent with the tongue being positioned towards the back of the mouth.

The International Phonetic Alphabet defines five degrees of vowel backness (sorted according to backness, with the top-most one being the front-most back and the bottom-most being the back-most):

To them may be added front-central and back-central, corresponding to the vertical lines separating central from front and back vowel spaces in several IPA diagrams. However, front-central and back-central may also be used as terms synonymous with near-front and near-back. No language is known to contrast more than three degrees of backness nor is there a language that contrasts front with near-front vowels nor back with near-back ones.

Although some English dialects have vowels at five degrees of backness, there is no known language that distinguishes five degrees of backness without additional differences in height or rounding.

Roundedness

[edit]

Roundedness is named after the rounding of the lips in some vowels. Because lip rounding is easily visible, vowels may be commonly identified as rounded based on the articulation of the lips. Acoustically, rounded vowels are identified chiefly by a decrease in F2, although F1 is also slightly decreased.

In most languages, roundedness is a reinforcing feature of mid to high back vowels rather than a distinctive feature. Usually, the higher a back vowel, the more intense is the rounding. However, in some languages, roundedness is independent from backness, such as French and German (with front rounded vowels), most Uralic languages (Estonian has a rounding contrast for /o/ and front vowels), Turkic languages (with a rounding distinction for front vowels and /u/), and Vietnamese with back unrounded vowels.

Nonetheless, even in those languages there is usually some phonetic correlation between rounding and backness: front rounded vowels tend to be more front-central than front, and back unrounded vowels tend to be more back-central than back. Thus, the placement of unrounded vowels to the left of rounded vowels on the IPA vowel chart is reflective of their position in formant space.

Different kinds of labialization are possible. In mid to high rounded back vowels the lips are generally protruded ("pursed") outward, a phenomenon known as endolabial rounding because the insides of the lips are visible, whereas in mid to high rounded front vowels the lips are generally "compressed" with the margins of the lips pulled in and drawn towards each other, a phenomenon known as exolabial rounding. However, not all languages follow that pattern. Japanese /u/, for example, is an exolabial (compressed) back vowel, and sounds quite different from an English endolabial /u/. Swedish and Norwegian are the only two known languages in which the feature is contrastive; they have both exo- and endo-labial close front vowels and close central vowels, respectively. In many phonetic treatments, both are considered types of rounding, but some phoneticians do not believe that these are subsets of a single phenomenon and posit instead three independent features of rounded (endolabial), compressed (exolabial), and unrounded. The lip position of unrounded vowels may also be classified separately as spread and neutral (neither rounded nor spread).[12] Others distinguish compressed rounded vowels, in which the corners of the mouth are drawn together, from compressed unrounded vowels, in which the lips are compressed but the corners remain apart as in spread vowels.

Front, raised and retracted

[edit]
Front, raised and retracted are the three articulatory dimensions of vowel space. Open and close refer to the jaw, not the tongue.

The conception of the tongue moving in two directions, high–low and front–back, is not supported by articulatory evidence and does not clarify how articulation affects vowel quality. Vowels may instead be characterized by the three directions of movement of the tongue from its neutral position: front (forward), raised (upward and back), and retracted (downward and back). Front vowels ([i, e, ?] and, to a lesser extent [?, ?, ?, ?], etc.), can be secondarily qualified as close or open, as in the traditional conception, but this refers to jaw rather than tongue position. In addition, rather than there being a unitary category of back vowels, the regrouping posits raised vowels, where the body of the tongue approaches the velum ([u, o, ?], etc.), and retracted vowels, where the root of the tongue approaches the pharynx ([ɑ, ?], etc.):

Membership in these categories is scalar, with the mid-central vowels being marginal to any category.[13]

Nasalization

[edit]

Nasalization occurs when air escapes through the nose. Vowels are often nasalised under the influence of neighbouring nasal consonants, as in English hand [h??nd]. Nasalised vowels, however, should not be confused with nasal vowels. The latter refers to vowels that are distinct from their oral counterparts, as in French /ɑ/ vs. /ɑ?/.[14]

In nasal vowels, the velum is lowered, and some air travels through the nasal cavity as well as the mouth. An oral vowel is a vowel in which all air escapes through the mouth. Polish and Portuguese also contrast nasal and oral vowels.

Phonation

[edit]

Voicing describes whether the vocal cords are vibrating during the articulation of a vowel. Most languages have only voiced vowels, but several Native American languages, such as Cheyenne and Totonac, have both voiced and devoiced vowels in complementary distribution. Vowels are devoiced in whispered speech. In Japanese and in Quebec French, vowels that are between voiceless consonants are often devoiced. Keres is disputed to have phonemic voiceless vowels but no language is confirmed to have them phonemically.

Modal voice, creaky voice, and breathy voice (murmured vowels) are phonation types that are used contrastively in some languages. Often, they co-occur with tone or stress distinctions; in the Mon language, vowels pronounced in the high tone are also produced with creaky voice. In such cases, it can be unclear whether it is the tone, the voicing type, or the pairing of the two that is being used for phonemic contrast. The combination of phonetic cues (phonation, tone, stress) is known as register or register complex.

Tenseness

[edit]

Tenseness is used to describe the opposition of tense vowels vs. lax vowels. This opposition has traditionally been thought to be a result of greater muscular tension, though phonetic experiments have repeatedly failed to show this.[citation needed]

Unlike the other features of vowel quality, tenseness is only applicable to the few languages that have this opposition (mainly Germanic languages, e.g. German), whereas the vowels of the other languages (e.g. Spanish) cannot be described with respect to tenseness in any meaningful way.[citation needed]

One may distinguish the English tense vs. lax vowels roughly, with its spelling. Tense vowels usually occur in words with the final silent ⟨e⟩, as in mate. Lax vowels occur in words without the silent ⟨e⟩, such as mat. In American English, lax vowels [?, ?, ?, ?, ?] do not appear in stressed open syllables.[15]

In traditional grammar, long vowels vs. short vowels are more commonly used, compared to tense and lax. The two sets of terms are used interchangeably by some because the features are concomitant in some varieties of English.[clarification needed] In most Germanic languages, lax vowels can only occur in closed syllables. Therefore, they are also known as checked vowels, whereas the tense vowels are called free vowels since they can occur in any kind of syllable.[citation needed]

Tongue root position

[edit]

Advanced tongue root (ATR) is a feature common across much of Africa, the Pacific Northwest, and scattered other languages such as Modern Mongolian.[16] The contrast between advanced and retracted tongue root resembles the tense-lax contrast acoustically, but they are articulated differently. Those vowels involve noticeable tension in the vocal tract.

Secondary narrowings in the vocal tract

[edit]

Pharyngealized vowels occur in some languages like Sedang and the Tungusic languages. Pharyngealisation is similar in articulation to retracted tongue root but is acoustically distinct.

A stronger degree of pharyngealisation occurs in the Northeast Caucasian languages and the Khoisan languages. They might be called epiglottalized since the primary constriction is at the tip of the epiglottis.

The greatest degree of pharyngealisation is found in the strident vowels of the Khoisan languages, where the larynx is raised, and the pharynx constricted, so that either the epiglottis or the arytenoid cartilages vibrate instead of the vocal cords.

The terms pharyngealized, epiglottalized, strident, and sphincteric are sometimes used interchangeably.

Rhotic vowels

[edit]

Rhotic vowels are the "R-colored vowels" of American English and a few other languages.

Reduced vowels

[edit]
Common reduced vowels
(IPA provides only ⟨?⟩ and ⟨?⟩)
Near-
front
Central Near-
back
Near-close ? ?
Mid ?
Near-open ?

Some languages, such as English and Russian, have what are called 'reduced', 'weak' or 'obscure' vowels in some unstressed positions. These do not correspond one-to-one with the vowel sounds that occur in stressed position (so-called 'full' vowels), and they tend to be mid-centralized in comparison, as well as having reduced rounding or spreading. The IPA has long provided two letters for obscure vowels, mid ⟨?⟩ and lower ⟨?⟩, neither of which are defined for rounding. Dialects of English may have up to four phonemic reduced vowels: /?/, /?/, and higher unrounded /?/ and rounded /?/. (The non-IPA letters ⟨?⟩ and ⟨?⟩ may be used for the latter to avoid confusion with the clearly defined values of IPA letters like ⟨?⟩ and ⟨?⟩, which are also seen, since the IPA only provides for two reduced vowels.)

Acoustics

[edit]
Spectrogram of vowels [i, u, ɑ]. [ɑ] is a low vowel, so its F1 value is higher than that of [i] and [u], which are high vowels. [i] is a front vowel, so its F2 is substantially higher than that of [u] and [ɑ], which are back vowels.
An idealized schematic of vowel space, based on the formants of Daniel Jones and John Wells pronouncing the cardinal vowels of the IPA. The scale is logarithmic. The grey range is where F2 would be less than F1, which by definition is impossible. [a] is an extra-low central vowel. Phonemically it may be front or back, depending on the language. Rounded vowels that are front in tongue position are front-central in formant space, while unrounded vowels that are back in articulation are back-central in formant space. Thus [y ?] have perhaps similar F1 and F2 values to the high central vowels [? ?], being distinguished by rounding (F3); similarly [? ?] vs central [? ?] and [? ?] vs central [? ?].
The same chart, with a few intermediate vowels. Low front [?] is intermediate between [a] and [?], while [?] is intermediate between [ɑ] and [?]. The back vowels change gradually in rounding, from unrounded [ɑ] and slightly rounded [?] to tightly rounded [u]; similarly slightly rounded [?] to tightly rounded [y]. With [a] seen as an (extra-)low central vowel, the vowels [? ? ɑ] can be redefined as front, central and back (near-)low vowels.

The acoustics of vowels are fairly well understood. The different vowel qualities are realized in acoustic analyses of vowels by the relative values of the formants, acoustic resonances of the vocal tract which show up as dark bands on a spectrogram. The vocal tract acts as a resonant cavity, and the position of the jaw, lips, and tongue affect the parameters of the resonant cavity, resulting in different formant values. The acoustics of vowels can be visualized using spectrograms, which display the acoustic energy at each frequency, and how this changes with time.

The first formant, abbreviated "F1", corresponds to vowel openness (vowel height). Open vowels have high F1 frequencies, while close vowels have low F1 frequencies, as can be seen in the accompanying spectrogram: The [i] and [u] have similar low first formants, whereas [ɑ] has a higher formant.

The second formant, F2, corresponds to vowel frontness. Back vowels have low F2 frequencies, while front vowels have high F2 frequencies. This is very clear in the spectrogram, where the front vowel [i] has a much higher F2 frequency than the other two vowels. However, in open vowels, the high F1 frequency forces a rise in the F2 frequency as well, so an alternative measure of frontness is the difference between the first and second formants. For this reason, some people prefer to plot as F1 vs. F2 – F1. (This dimension is usually called 'backness' rather than 'frontness', but the term 'backness' can be counterintuitive when discussing formants.)

In the third edition of his textbook, Peter Ladefoged recommended using plots of F1 against F2 – F1 to represent vowel quality.[17] However, in the fourth edition, he changed to adopt a simple plot of F1 against F2,[18] and this simple plot of F1 against F2 was maintained for the fifth (and final) edition of the book.[19] Katrina Hayward compares the two types of plots and concludes that plotting of F1 against F2 – F1 "is not very satisfactory because of its effect on the placing of the central vowels",[20] so she also recommends use of a simple plot of F1 against F2. In fact, this kind of plot of F1 against F2 has been used by analysts to show the quality of the vowels in a wide range of languages, including RP,[21][22] the Queen's English,[23] American English,[24] Singapore English,[25] Brunei English,[26] North Frisian,[27] Turkish Kabardian,[28] and various indigenous Australian languages.[29]

R-colored vowels are characterized by lowered F3 values.

Rounding is generally realized by a decrease of F2 that tends to reinforce vowel backness. One effect of this is that back vowels are most commonly rounded while front vowels are most commonly unrounded; another is that rounded vowels tend to plot to the right of unrounded vowels in vowel charts. That is, there is a reason for plotting vowel pairs the way they are.

Prosody and intonation

[edit]

In addition to variation in vowel quality as described above, vowels vary as a result of differences in prosody. The most important prosodic variables are pitch (fundamental frequency), loudness (intensity) and length (duration). However, the features of prosody are usually considered to apply not to the vowel itself, but to the syllable in which the vowel occurs. In other words, the domain of prosody is the syllable, not the segment (vowel or consonant).[30] We can list briefly the effect of prosody on the vowel component of a syllable.

  • Pitch: in the case of a syllable such as 'cat', the only voiced portion of the syllable is the vowel, so the vowel carries the pitch information. This may relate to the syllable in which it occurs, or to a larger stretch of speech to which an intonation contour belongs. In a word such as 'man', all the segments in the syllable are sonorant and all will participate in any pitch variation.
  • Loudness: this variable has been traditionally associated with linguistic stress, though other factors are usually involved in this. Lehiste (ibid) argues that stress, or loudness, could not be associated with a single segment in a syllable independently of the rest of the syllable (p. 147). This means that vowel loudness is a concomitant of the loudness of the syllable in which it occurs.
  • Length: it is important to distinguish two aspects of vowel length. One is the phonological difference in length exhibited by some languages. Japanese, Finnish, Hungarian, Arabic and Latin have a two-way phonemic contrast between short and long vowels. The Mixe language has a three-way contrast among short, half-long, and long vowels.[31] The other type of length variation in vowels is non-distinctive, and is the result of prosodic variation in speech: vowels tend to be lengthened when in a stressed syllable, or when utterance rate is slow.

Monophthongs, diphthongs, triphthongs

[edit]

A vowel sound whose quality does not change throughout the vowel is called a monophthong. Monophthongs are sometimes called "pure" or "stable" vowels. A vowel sound that glides from one quality to another is called a diphthong, and a vowel sound that glides successively through three qualities is a triphthong.

All languages have monophthongs and many languages have diphthongs, but triphthongs or vowel sounds with even more target qualities are relatively rare cross-linguistically. English has all three types: the vowel sound in hit is a monophthong /?/, the vowel sound in boy is in most dialects a diphthong /??/, and the vowel sounds of flower, /a??r/, form a triphthong or disyllable, depending on the dialect.

In phonology, diphthongs and triphthongs are distinguished from sequences of monophthongs by whether the vowel sound may be analyzed into distinct phonemes. For example, the vowel sounds in a two-syllable pronunciation of the word flower (/?fla??r/) phonetically form a disyllabic triphthong but are phonologically a sequence of a diphthong (represented by the letters ⟨ow⟩) and a monophthong (represented by the letters ⟨er⟩). Some linguists use the terms diphthong and triphthong only in this phonemic sense.

Written vowels

[edit]

The name "vowel" is often used for the symbols that represent vowel sounds in a language's writing system, particularly if the language uses an alphabet. In writing systems based on the Latin alphabet, the letters ⟨a⟩, ⟨e⟩, ⟨i⟩, ⟨o⟩, ⟨u⟩, ⟨y⟩, ⟨w⟩ and sometimes others can all be used to represent vowels. However, not all of these letters represent the vowels in all languages that use this writing, or even consistently within one language. Some of them, especially ⟨w⟩ and ⟨y⟩, are also used to represent approximant consonants. Moreover, a vowel might be represented by a letter usually reserved for consonants, or a combination of letters, particularly where one letter represents several sounds at once, or vice versa; examples from English include ⟨igh⟩ in "thigh" and ⟨x⟩ in "x-ray". In addition, extensions of the Latin alphabet have such independent vowel letters as ⟨?⟩, ⟨?⟩, ⟨?⟩, ⟨?⟩, and ⟨?⟩.

The phonetic values vary considerably by language, and some languages use ⟨i⟩ and ⟨y⟩ for the consonant [j], e.g., initial ⟨i⟩ in Italian or Romanian and initial ⟨y⟩ in English. In the original Latin alphabet, there was no written distinction between ⟨v⟩ and ⟨u⟩, and the letter represented the approximant [w] and the vowels [u] and [?]. In Modern Welsh, ⟨w⟩ represents these same sounds. There is not necessarily a direct one-to-one correspondence between the vowel sounds of a language and the vowel letters. Many languages that use a form of the Latin alphabet have more vowel sounds than can be represented by the standard set of five vowel letters. In English spelling, the five letters ⟨a⟩ ⟨e⟩ ⟨i⟩ ⟨o⟩ and ⟨u⟩ can represent a variety of vowel sounds, while the letter ⟨y⟩ frequently represents vowels (as in e.g., "gym", "happy", or the diphthongs in "cry", "thyme");[32] ⟨w⟩ is used in representing some diphthongs (as in "cow") and to represent a monophthong in the borrowed words "cwm" and "crwth" (sometimes cruth).

Other languages cope with the limitation in the number of Latin vowel letters in similar ways. Many languages make extensive use of combinations of letters to represent various sounds. Other languages use vowel letters with modifications, such as ⟨?⟩ in Swedish, or add diacritical marks, like ogoneks, to vowels to represent the variety of possible vowel sounds. Some languages have also constructed additional vowel letters by modifying the standard Latin vowels in other ways, such as ⟨?⟩ or ⟨?⟩ that are found in some of the Scandinavian languages. The International Phonetic Alphabet has a set of 28 symbols representing the range of essential vowel qualities, and a further set of diacritics to denote variations from the basic vowel.

The writing systems used for some languages, such as the Hebrew alphabet and the Arabic alphabet, do not ordinarily mark all the vowels, since they are frequently unnecessary in identifying a word. [citation needed] Technically, these are called abjads rather than alphabets. Although it is possible to construct English sentences that can be understood without written vowels (cn y rd ths?), single words in English lacking written vowels can be indistinguishable; consider dd, which could be any of dad, dada, dado, dead, deed, did, died, diode, dodo, dud, dude, odd, add, and aided. (Abjads generally express some word-internal vowels and all word-initial and word-final vowels, whereby the ambiguity will be much reduced.) The Masoretes devised a vowel notation system for Hebrew Jewish scripture that is still widely used, as well as the trope symbols used for its cantillation; both are part of oral tradition and still the basis for many bible translations—Jewish and Christian.

Shifts

[edit]

The differences in pronunciation of vowel letters between English and its related languages can be accounted for by the Great Vowel Shift. After printing was introduced to England, and therefore after spelling was more or less standardized, a series of dramatic changes in the pronunciation of the vowel phonemes occurred, and continued into recent centuries, but were not reflected in the spelling system. This has led to numerous inconsistencies in the spelling of English vowel sounds and the pronunciation of English vowel letters (and to the mispronunciation of foreign words and names by speakers of English).

Audio samples

[edit]

Systems

[edit]

The importance of vowels in distinguishing one word from another varies from language to language. Nearly all languages have at least three phonemic vowels, usually /i/, /a/, /u/ as in Classical Arabic, some Malayic languages of Borneo (including Banjarese) and Inuktitut, though Adyghe and many Sepik languages have a vertical vowel system of /?/, /?/, /a/. Very few languages have fewer, though some Arrernte, Circassian, and Ndu languages have been argued to have just two, /?/ and /a/, with [?] being epenthetic.

It is not straightforward to say which language has the most vowels, since that depends on how they are counted. For example, long vowels, nasal vowels, and various phonations may or may not be counted separately; indeed, it may sometimes be unclear if phonation belongs to the vowels or the consonants of a language. If such things are ignored and only vowels with dedicated IPA letters ('vowel qualities') are considered, then very few languages have more than ten. The Germanic languages have some of the largest inventories: Standard Danish has 11 to 13 short vowels (/(a), ɑ, (?), e, ?, ?, i, o, ?, u, ?, ?, y/), while the Amstetten dialect of Bavarian has been reported to have thirteen long vowels: /i, y, e, ?, ?, ?, ?, ?, a, ?, ?, o, u/.[citation needed] The situation can be quite disparate within a same family language: Spanish and French are two closely related Romance languages but Spanish has only five pure vowel qualities, /a, e, i, o, u/, while classical French has eleven: /a, ɑ, e, ?, i, o, ?, u, y, ?, ?/ and four nasal vowels /ɑ?/, /??/, /??/ and /??/. The Mon–Khmer languages of Southeast Asia also have some large inventories, such as the ten vowels of Khmer: /i, ?, e, ?, a, ɑ, ?, o, u, ?/. Wu dialects have the largest inventories of Chinese; the Jinhui dialect of Wu has also been reported to have eleven vowels: ten basic vowels, /i, y, e, ?, ?, ɑ, ?, o, u, ?/, plus restricted /?/; this does not count the seven nasal vowels.[33]

One of the most common vowels is [a?]; it is nearly universal for a language to have at least one open vowel, though most dialects of English have an [?] and a [ɑ]—and often an [?], all open vowels—but no central [a]. Some Tagalog and Cebuano speakers have [?] rather than [a], and Dhangu Yolngu is described as having /? ? ?/, without any peripheral vowels. [i] is also extremely common, though Tehuelche has just the vowels /e a o/ with no close vowels. The third vowel of the Arabic-type three-vowel system, /u/, is considerably less common. A large fraction of the languages of North America happen to have a four-vowel system without /u/: /i, e, a, o/; Nahuatl and Navajo are examples.

In most languages, vowels serve mainly to distinguish separate lexemes, rather than different inflectional forms of the same lexeme as they commonly do in the Semitic languages. For example, while English man becomes men in the plural, moon is a completely different word.

Words without vowels

[edit]

In rhotic dialects of English, as in Canada and the United States, there are many words such as bird, learn, girl, church, worst, worm, myrrh that some phoneticians analyze as having no vowels, only a syllabic consonant /??/. However, others analyze these words instead as having a rhotic vowel, /??/. The difference may be partially one of dialect.

There are a few such words that are disyllabic, like cursor, curtain, and turtle: [?k??s??], [?k??tn?] and [?t??tl?] (or [?k??s?], [?k??t?n], and [?t??t?l]), and even a few that are trisyllabic, at least in some accents, such as purpler [?p??.pl?.??], hurdler [?h??.dl?.??], gurgler [?ɡ??.ɡl?.??], and certainer [?s??.tn?.??].

The word and frequently contracts to a simple nasal ’n, as in lock 'n key [?l?k ? ?ki?]. Words such as will, have, and is regularly contract to ’ll [l], ’ve [v], and 's [z]. However, none of them are pronounced alone without vowels, so they are not phonological words. Onomatopoeic words that can be pronounced alone, and that have no vowels or ars, include hmm, pst!, shh!, tsk!, and zzz. As in other languages, onomatopoeiae stand outside the normal phonotactics of English.

There are other languages that form lexical words without vowel sounds. In Serbo-Croatian, for example, the consonants [r] and [r?] (the difference is not written) can act as a syllable nucleus and carry rising or falling tone; examples include the tongue-twister na vrh brda vrba mrda and geographic names such as Krk. In Czech and Slovak, either [l] or [r] can stand in for vowels: vlk [vl?k] "wolf", krk [kr?k] "neck". A particularly long word without vowels is ?tvrthrst, meaning "quarter-handful", with two syllables (one for each R), or scvrnkls, a verb form meaning "you flipped (sth) down" (eg a marble). Whole sentences (usually tongue-twisters) can be made from such words, such as Str? prst skrz krk, meaning "stick a finger through your neck" (pronounced [str?t? pr?st skr?s kr?k] ?), and Smr? pln skvrn zvlhl z mlh, which means "A morel full of spots wetted from fogs". (Here zvlhl has two syllables based on L; and the preposition z consists of a single consonant. Only prepositions do this in Czech, and they normally link phonetically to the following word, so not really behave as vowelless words.) In Russian, there are also prepositions that consist of a single consonant letter, like k, 'to', v, 'in', and s, 'with'. However, these forms are actually contractions of ko, vo, and so respectively, and these forms are still used in modern Russian before words with certain consonant clusters for ease of pronunciation.

In Kazakh and certain other Turkic languages, words without vowel sounds may occur due to reduction of weak vowels. A common example is the Kazakh word for one: bir, pronounced [br]. Among careful speakers, however, the original vowel may be preserved, and the vowels are always preserved in the orthography.

In Southern varieties of Chinese, such as Cantonese and Minnan, some monosyllabic words are made of exclusively nasals, such as Cantonese [m???] "no" and [????] "five". Minnan also has words consisting of a consonant followed by a syllabic nasal, such as pn?g "cooked rice".

So far, all of these syllabic consonants, at least in the lexical words, have been sonorants, such as [r], [l], [m], and [n], which have a voiced quality similar to vowels. (They can carry tone, for example.) However, there are languages with lexical words that not only contain no vowels, but contain no sonorants at all, like (non-lexical) shh! in English. These include some Berber languages, some languages of the American Pacific Northwest, such as Nuxalk, and some Northwest Caucasian languages, such as Abaza language. An example from Nuxalk is scs "seal fat" (pronounced [sxs], as spelled), and a longer one is clhp'xwlhtlhplhhskwts' (pronounced [x?p?χ??t??p???sk??t?s?]) "he had had in his possession a bunchberry plant". (Follow the Nuxalk link for other examples.) Berber examples include /tkkststt/ "you took it off" and /tfktstt/ "you gave it". Some words may contain one or two consonants only: /ɡ/ "be", /ks/ "feed on".[34]

Abaza language often drops word-final /?/ when forming compounds, making combinations such as хък?хвбкъвылк? /q?k?χ?bq???lk?/ "five vats of sour cream" possible. Therefore, even though the dictionary forms хък?ы /q?k??/ "sour cream" and хвпа /χ?p?a/ "five" have a schwa at the end, the consonant cluster /q?k?/ and the sole consonant /χ?/ (-pa is a counting suffix) can be analysed as the full word, with final vowels probably being a result of obligatory nucleus (such as in the first example, where the word бкъвыл /bq???l/ "vat" is a closed syllable, preventing the vowel deletion, unless /l/ is analysed as a syllabic consonant, which would still make it a nucleus but would make the word vowelless).

In Mandarin Chinese, words and syllables such as and zhī are sometimes described as being syllabic fricatives and affricates phonemically, /?/ and /t??/, but these do have a voiced segment that carries the tone.

In the Japonic language Miyako, there are words with no voiced sounds, such as ss 'dust', kss 'breast/milk', pss 'day', ff 'a comb', kff 'to make', fks 'to build', ksks 'month', sks 'to cut', psks 'to pull'.

Some analyses of Wandala is reported to have no phonemic vowels.[35]

Words consisting of only vowels

[edit]

It is not uncommon for short grammatical words to consist of only vowels, such as a and I in English. Lexical words are somewhat rarer in English and are generally restricted to a single syllable: eye, awe, owe, and in non-rhotic accents air, ore, err. Vowel-only words of more than one syllable are generally foreign loans, such as ai (two syllables: /?ɑ?i/) for the maned sloth, or proper names, such as Iowa (in some accents: /?a?.o?.?/).

However, vowel sequences in hiatus are more freely allowed in some other languages, most famously perhaps in Bantu and Polynesian languages, but also in Japanese and Finnic languages. In such languages there tends to be a larger variety of vowel-only words. In Swahili (Bantu), for example, there is aua 'to survey' and eua 'to purify' (both three syllables); in Japanese, aoi 青い 'blue/green' and oioi 追々 'gradually' (three and four morae); and in Finnish, aie 'intention' and auo 'open!' (both two syllables), although some dialects pronounce them as aije and auvo. In Urdu, āye/aaie ???? or āyn ???? 'come' is used. Hawaiian, and the Polynesian languages generally, have unusually large numbers of such words, such as aeāea (a small green fish), which is three syllables: ae.āe.a. Most long words involve reduplication, which is quite productive in Polynesian: ioio 'grooves', eaea 'breath', uaua 'tough' (all four syllables), auēuē 'crying' (five syllables, from uē (uwē) 'to weep'), uoa or uouoa 'false mullet' (sp. fish, three or five syllables).[citation needed]

See also

[edit]

Notes

[edit]
  1. ^ According to Peter Ladefoged, traditional articulatory descriptions such as height and backness "are not entirely satisfactory", and when phoneticians describe a vowel as high or low, they are in fact describing an acoustic quality rather than the actual position of the tongue.[11]

References

[edit]
  1. ^ Ladefoged & Maddieson 1996, p. 281.
  2. ^ "Vowel". Online Etymology Dictionary. Retrieved 12 April 2012.
  3. ^ Dictionary.com: vowel
  4. ^ Cruttenden, Alan (2014). Gimson's Pronunciation of English (Eighth ed.). Routledge. p. 27. ISBN 9781444183092.
  5. ^ Cruttenden, Alan (2014). Gimson's Pronunciation of English (Eighth ed.). Routledge. p. 53. ISBN 9781444183092.
  6. ^ Laver, John (1994) Principles of Phonetics, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, p. 269.
  7. ^ Crystal, David (2005) A Dictionary of Linguistics & Phonetics (Fifth Edition), Maldern, MA/Oxford: Blackwell, p. 494.
  8. ^ Ladefoged, Peter; Maddieson, Ian (1996). The Sounds of the World's Languages. Oxford: Blackwell. p. 323. ISBN 0-631-19815-6.
  9. ^ Ladefoged & Disner (2012) Vowels and Consonants, 3rd ed., p. 132.
  10. ^ IPA (1999) Handbook of the IPA, p. 12.
  11. ^ Ladefoged, Peter (2006) A Course in Phonetics (Fifth Edition), Boston, MA: Thomson Wadsworth, p. 189.
  12. ^ IPA (1999), p. 13.
  13. ^ John Esling (2005) "There Are No Back Vowels: The Laryngeal Articulator Model", The Canadian Journal of Linguistics 50: 13–44
  14. ^ Botma, Bert (8 December 2020). "Nasals and Nasalization". Oxford Bibliographies. Oxford. Retrieved 27 June 2017.
  15. ^ Ladefoged, Peter & Johnson, Keith. (2011). Tense and Lax Vowels. In A Course in Phonetics (6th ed., pp. 98–100). Boston, MA: Cengage.
  16. ^ Bessell, Nicola J. (1993). Towards a phonetic and phonological typology of post-velar articulation (Thesis). University of British Columbia.
  17. ^ Ladefoged, Peter (1993) A Course in Phonetics (Third Edition), Fort Worth: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich, p. 197.
  18. ^ Ladefoged, Peter (2001) A Course in Phonetics (Fourth Edition), Fort Worth: Harcourt, p. 177.
  19. ^ Ladefoged, Peter (2006) A Course in Phonetics (Fifth Edition), Boston: Thomson Wadsworth, p. 189.
  20. ^ Hayward, Katrina (2000) Experimental Phonetics, Harlow, UK: Pearson, p. 160.
  21. ^ Deterding, David (1997). "The formants of monophthong vowels in Standard Southern British English Pronunciation". Journal of the International Phonetic Association. 27 (1–2): 47–55. doi:10.1017/S0025100300005417. S2CID 146157247.
  22. ^ Hawkins, Sarah and Jonathan Midgley (2005). "Formant frequencies of RP monophthongs in four age groups of speakers". Journal of the International Phonetic Association. 35 (2): 183–199. doi:10.1017/S0025100305002124. S2CID 53532910.
  23. ^ Harrington, Jonathan, Sallyanne Palethorpe and Catherine Watson (2005) Deepening or lessening the divide between diphthongs: an analysis of the Queen's annual Christmas broadcasts. In William J. Hardcastle and Janet Mackenzie Beck (eds.) A Figure of Speech: A Festschrift for John Laver, Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum, pp. 227-261.
  24. ^ Flemming, Edward and Stephanie Johnson (2007). "Rosa's roses: reduced vowels in American English" (PDF). Journal of the International Phonetic Association. 37: 83–96. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.536.1989. doi:10.1017/S0025100306002817. S2CID 145535175.
  25. ^ Deterding, David (2003). "An instrumental study of the monophthong vowels of Singapore English". English World-Wide. 24: 1–16. doi:10.1075/eww.24.1.02det.
  26. ^ Salbrina, Sharbawi (2006). "The vowels of Brunei English: an acoustic investigation". English World-Wide. 27 (3): 247–264. doi:10.1075/eww.27.3.03sha.
  27. ^ Bohn, Ocke-Schwen (2004). "How to organize a fairly large vowel inventory: the vowels of Fering (North Frisian)" (PDF). Journal of the International Phonetic Association. 34 (2): 161–173. doi:10.1017/S002510030400180X. S2CID 59404078.
  28. ^ Gordon, Matthew and Ayla Applebaum (2006). "Phonetic structures of Turkish Kabardian" (PDF). Journal of the International Phonetic Association. 36 (2): 159–186. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.233.1206. doi:10.1017/S0025100306002532. S2CID 6665600.
  29. ^ Fletcher, Janet (2006) Exploring the phonetics of spoken narratives in Australian indigenous languages. In William J. Hardcastle and Janet Mackenzie Beck (eds.) A Figure of Speech: A Festschrift for John Laver, Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum, pp. 201–226.
  30. ^ Lehiste, Ilse, Suprasegmentals, M.I.T 1970, pp. 42, 84, 147
  31. ^ Ladefoged, P. and Maddieson, I. The Sounds of the World's Languages, Blackwell (1996), p 320
  32. ^ In wyrm and myrrh, there is neither a vowel letter nor, in rhotic dialects, a vowel sound.
  33. ^ Values in open oral syllables Archived 2025-08-14 at the Wayback Machine
  34. ^ Audio recordings of selected words without vowels can be downloaded from "audio.doc". Archived from the original on 2025-08-14. Retrieved 2025-08-14..
  35. ^ Wolff, H. Ekkehard. "'Vocalogenesis' in (Central) Chadic languages" (PDF). Retrieved 2 December 2017.

Bibliography

[edit]
[edit]
Listen to this article (18 minutes)
Spoken Wikipedia icon
This audio file was created from a revision of this article dated 18 July 2005 (2025-08-14), and does not reflect subsequent edits.
东山再起是什么生肖 三七粉主治什么 安哥拉树皮有什么功效 血吸虫是什么动物 5月5是什么星座
小孩查微量元素挂什么科 vcr是什么意思 西米是什么做的 奴才是什么意思 减脂吃什么
有鳞状细胞是什么意思 什么颜色加什么颜色等于灰色 28岁属什么的 医学ace是什么意思 割包皮是什么
肺结节挂什么科 脾胃气虚吃什么中成药 莲白是什么菜 淡定自若什么意思 夏五行属什么
社会公德的主要内容是什么hcv8jop4ns9r.cn 黄皮果什么时候成熟hcv7jop6ns7r.cn 刘亦菲是什么国籍hcv8jop4ns3r.cn 咖啡豆是什么动物粪便hcv7jop6ns2r.cn 诺如病毒是什么hcv9jop3ns2r.cn
有才是什么意思hcv9jop6ns5r.cn 韩语思密达是什么意思hcv7jop6ns1r.cn 肋骨下面是什么部位helloaicloud.com 山茱萸有什么功效hcv9jop1ns5r.cn 额头窄适合什么发型hcv9jop3ns7r.cn
骨龄什么时候闭合hcv9jop0ns0r.cn 查岗是什么意思dajiketang.com 益生元和益生菌有什么区别hcv8jop2ns4r.cn 初中什么时候开学hcv9jop6ns6r.cn 王秋儿和王冬儿什么关系hcv8jop1ns4r.cn
痔疮便血吃什么药hcv8jop1ns5r.cn 气血亏虚吃什么中成药hcv8jop5ns3r.cn 男宝胶囊为什么不建议吃beikeqingting.com 肚子容易胀气是什么原因hcv9jop7ns2r.cn 捡尸是什么意思hcv9jop4ns8r.cn
百度