荷叶是什么的什么| 晚上七点到九点是什么时辰| 什么情况下需做肠镜| bdsm什么意思| 怕冷不怕热是什么体质| 脚为什么会抽筋| 芈月传芈姝结局是什么| 香菜什么时候种植最好| 扁桃体肥大是什么原因造成的| 葡萄糖有什么作用| 小孩脸肿是什么原因引起的| 怀男孩和女孩有什么区别| 梦到女儿死了是什么意思| 霍金什么病| 正月二十一是什么星座| 蒸蒸日上什么意思| 什么的走路| 咳嗽咳白痰是什么症状| 左手麻木是什么原因引起的| 勾心斗角什么意思| 梦见摘西红柿是什么意思| 出佛身血是什么意思| 专科警校出来干什么| 鲟鱼吃什么| 金骏眉茶是什么茶| 水洗真丝是什么面料| 湿热体质吃什么中成药| 玉米淀粉可以做什么| 财源广进是什么生肖| 辅酶q10是什么| 下巴发黑是什么原因| 北京有什么好玩的地方| 熬中药用什么锅最好| 星期一左眼皮跳是什么预兆| 蛇爱吃什么食物| 前面有个豹子是什么车| 日本豆腐是用什么做的| 急性荨麻疹用什么药| 皂角是什么| 为什么得甲亢| 掉头发吃什么恢复最快| 着床出血什么时候出现| 或缺是什么意思| 顾里为什么和席城睡了| 247是什么意思| 皮蛋为什么含铅| 苏轼是什么朝代的| 鱼露是什么东西| 工口什么意思| 什么是商k| 消渴是什么意思| 美满霉素又叫什么名字| 为什么要吃叶酸| 吃什么代谢快有助于减肥| 美国报警电话为什么是911| 肟是什么意思| 孩子不好好吃饭是什么原因| oc是什么| 牛头不对马嘴是什么意思| 开天门是什么意思| 微信证件号是什么| 考研复试考什么| 尿路感染是什么症状| 食是代表什么生肖| 三点水开念什么意思| 犯罪是什么意思| 喘不上来气是什么原因| 大面积杀跳蚤用什么药| 什么什么的落叶| 纯粹的人是什么性格| 0和1是什么意思| 爱是什么东西| 市级三好学生有什么用| 传媒公司是干什么的| 牙齿最多的动物是什么| 白牡丹属于什么茶| 脸上浮肿是什么原因导致的| 宝宝肠胃炎吃什么药| 冰箱底部漏水是什么原因| 副主任医师什么级别| 砥砺是什么意思| h是什么牌子的皮带| 凌晨3点是什么时辰| 洪都拉斯为什么不与中国建交| 俄罗斯的货币叫什么| 血脂高吃什么水果最好| 什么人容易得梦游症| 思维什么意思| 失眠用什么药好| 病案首页是什么| 什么食物含叶黄素最多| 一个火一个华念什么| acer是什么牌子| 花枝招展什么意思| 什么什么的草地| 胃糜烂和胃溃疡有什么区别| 健胃消食片什么时候吃最好| 睡觉多梦是什么原因引起的| 李倩梅结局是什么| 兰蔻适合什么年龄的人用| 女性尿频尿急挂什么科| 什么是碱性食物| 女用避孕套是什么样的| 杭盖是什么意思| 监护是什么意思| 男人前列腺在什么位置| 室内用什么隔墙最便宜| 2000年出生属什么| 手术拆线挂什么科| 梦到鞋子是什么意思| 口腔溃疡吃什么药| 心脏彩超能查出什么| 纳豆是什么味道| 乳腺结节什么症状表现| 丰的部首是什么偏旁| 梧桐树的叶子像什么| 阴郁是什么意思| 泽五行属什么| 宰相相当于现在什么官| 重庆是什么地形| 狗狗胰腺炎有什么症状| 快递客服主要做什么| 盐酸二甲双胍缓释片什么时候吃| 什么是复句| 湖南有什么景点| 小孩头疼是什么原因| got什么意思| 什么是交感神经| 三只手是什么意思| 天丝是什么面料| 七月五号是什么星座| 宫腔灌注是治疗什么的| 昱五行属性是什么| 水泊梁山什么意思| 太监是什么意思| 据悉是什么意思| 香五行属什么| 煲汤用什么锅最好| 慎重考虑是什么意思| 休学是什么意思| 感冒喝什么饮料| 杭字五行属什么| 涤棉是什么材质| 天热出汗多是什么原因| 用盐洗脸有什么好处| 梦见打老婆是什么预兆| 忠心不二是什么生肖| raf是什么意思| 梦见抓蝎子是什么意思| 脱敏是什么意思| 鳜鱼是什么鱼| 85年五行属什么| 瓜尔佳氏现在姓什么| 宫腔内钙化灶是什么意思| 文科女生学什么专业好| 什么是生物制剂药| 手掌心有痣代表什么| 阿尔茨海默病吃什么药| 内眼角越揉越痒用什么眼药水| 远字五行属什么| 前列腺肥大吃什么药效果最好| 额头出油多是什么原因| 痰多吃什么化痰| 平反是什么意思| 什么叫两会| 臻的意思是什么| 挂号信什么意思| 小学生的学籍号是什么| 肺炎衣原体和支原体有什么区别| 眼睛红是什么原因引起的| 睡眠瘫痪症是什么| 生吃大蒜有什么好处| 梦见移坟墓是什么预兆| 睡觉被口水呛醒是什么原因| 干什么挣钱快| 什么是遗精| 省委巡视组组长什么级别| 熊猫喜欢吃什么食物| 周朝之后是什么朝代| junior是什么意思| 狼狗是什么品种| 北顶娘娘庙求什么灵验| 黄色配什么颜色| 嗓子有粘痰什么原因| 尿酸高不能吃什么| 梦见自己理发是什么意思| 木耳菜不能和什么一起吃| 属马的贵人属相是什么| 陕西有什么烟| 小孩子记忆力差是什么原因| 哈气是什么意思| 低血压吃什么可以补| 650是什么意思| 过敏有什么症状| 多出汗是什么原因| 张牙舞爪是什么生肖| 吃不胖是什么原因| 虱目鱼在大陆叫什么| 屁股又叫什么| 铁蛋白高挂什么科| 乌托邦是什么意思| ca19-9偏高是什么意思| 花开富贵是什么生肖| 丝瓜配什么炒好吃| 梗塞是什么意思| 什么炒鸡蛋| 心脏彩超能检查出什么| 驻马店以前叫什么名字| 心电图能检查出什么| 化验大便能查出什么病| 血冲脑是什么原因引起| 什么是题材股| 吃什么除湿气| 超度什么意思| 小孩睡觉出很多汗是什么原因| 男性生殖器叫什么| 炖牛肉放什么调料| 意识是什么意思| 没字去掉三点水念什么| 卧底归来大结局是什么| 性功能减退吃什么药好| 忘带洗面奶用什么代替| 韦编三绝是什么意思| 高血糖吃什么水果最好| 甲亢吃什么好的更快| 百折不挠指什么生肖| 降7是什么调| 酸根是什么| 冷漠是什么意思| 冥冥之中是什么意思| blood什么意思| 容易出汗是什么问题| baron是什么意思| 别来无恙什么意思| 噻虫高氯氟治什么虫| 坐久了脚肿是什么原因| 着相什么意思| 头昏吃什么药| 施华洛世奇水晶是什么材质| 肝阳上亢是什么意思| 晕3d是什么原因| 混圈是什么意思| 梦见邻居是什么意思| 微信证件号是什么| 头晕是什么感觉| 鬼畜是什么意思| 孕妇感冒了可以吃什么药| 碱性磷酸酶偏低是什么原因| 缀化是什么意思| 胸部b超挂什么科| 蚕豆病是什么病| 血瘀吃什么中成药| 坐围和臀围有什么区别| 囤货是什么意思| 维生素d3什么牌子好| 申时出生五行缺什么| ost是什么意思| 党内的最高处分是什么| 盐糖水有什么功效作用| 梦见经血是什么预兆| 不打紧是什么意思| 泉中水是什么生肖| 掉头发是什么原因男性| 红枣不能和什么一起吃| 百度Jump to content

硬度不够吃什么药调理

From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia
Cartoon of a child sitting in a cart hitched to a much smaller toy horse, as if expecting the horse to pull her along
Illustration from St. Nicholas: an Illustrated Magazine for Young Folks (1884) of a child imagining that a small, toy horse might pull his cart
百度 ”这是1997年张佐良大夫在周恩来生前副卫士长张树迎家中对笔者讲述的。

Wishful thinking is the formation of beliefs based on what might be pleasing to imagine, rather than on evidence, rationality, or reality. It is a product of resolving conflicts between belief and desire.[1] Methodologies to examine wishful thinking are diverse. Various disciplines and schools of thought examine related mechanisms such as neural circuitry, human cognition and emotion, types of bias, procrastination, motivation, optimism, attention and environment. This concept has been examined as a fallacy. It is related to the concept of wishful seeing.

Some psychologists believe that positive thinking is able to positively influence behavior and so bring about better results. This is called the "Pygmalion effect".[2][3] Studies have consistently shown that holding all else equal, subjects will have unrealistic optimism and predict positive outcomes to be more likely than negative outcomes. Research also suggests that under certain circumstances, such as when threat increases, a reverse phenomenon occurs.[4]

As a fallacy

[edit]

In addition to being a cognitive bias and a poor way of making decisions, wishful thinking is commonly held to be a specific informal fallacy in an argument when it is assumed that because we wish something to be true or false, it is actually true or false. This fallacy has the form "I wish that P were true/false; therefore, P is true/false."[5] Wishful thinking, if this were true, would rely upon appeals to emotion, and would also be a red herring.[6]

Wishful thinking may cause blindness to unintended consequences.[7]

Wishful seeing

[edit]

Wishful seeing is the phenomenon in which a person's internal state influences their visual perception. People have the tendency to believe that they perceive the world for what it is, but research suggests otherwise. Currently, there are two main types of wishful seeing based on where wishful seeing occurs—in categorization of objects or in representations of an environment.[4]

The concept of wishful seeing was first introduced by the New Look approach to psychology. The New Look approach was popularized in the 1950s through the work of Jerome Bruner and Cecile Goodman. In their classic 1947 study, they asked children to demonstrate their perception of the size of coins by manipulating the diameter of a circular aperture on a wooden box. Each child held the coin in their left hand at the same height and distance from the aperture and operated the knob to change the size of the aperture with their right hand. The children were divided into three groups, two experimental and one control, with ten children in each group. The control group was asked to estimate the size of coin-sized cardboard discs instead of actual coins. On average, the children of the experimental groups overestimated the size of the coins by thirty percent. In a second iteration of the experiment, Bruner and Goodman divided the children into groups based on economic status. Again, both the "poor" and "rich" groups were asked to estimate the size of real coins by manipulating the diameter of the aperture. As was expected, both groups overestimated the size of the coins, but the "poor" group overestimated the size by as much as fifty percent, which was up to thirty percent more than the "rich" group. From these results Bruner and Goodman concluded that poorer children felt a greater desire for money and thus perceived the coins as larger. This hypothesis formed the basis of the New Look psychological approach which suggests that the subjective experience of an object influences the visual perception of that object.[8] Some psychodynamic psychologists adopted the views of the New Look approach in order to explain how individuals might protect themselves from disturbing visual stimuli. The psychodynamic perspective lost support because it lacked a sufficient model to account for how the unconscious could influence perception.[9]

Although some further research was able to replicate the results found by Bruner and Goodman, the New Look approach was mostly abandoned by the 1970s because the experiments were riddled with methodological errors that did not account for confounding factors such as reporter bias and context.[10] Recent research has brought about a revival of New Look perspectives, but with methodological improvements to resolve the outstanding issues that plagued the original studies.[9]

Reverse wishful thinking and seeing

[edit]

This process occurs when threat increases.[4] The Ebbinghaus illusion has been used to measure reverse wishful seeing, with participants observing negative flanker targets underestimated less than positive or neutral targets.[11] Feelings of fear also lead to perception of the feared object as closer just as prior research suggests that desired objects are perceived as closer.[12] Furthermore, some people are less inclined to wishful thinking/seeing based on their emotional states or personality.[4]

Underlying mechanisms

[edit]

Cognition

[edit]

Concrete cognitive mechanisms underlying wishful thinking and wishful seeing are unknown. Since these concepts are still developing, research on the mechanisms contributing to this phenomenon is still in progress. However, some mechanisms have been proposed. Wishful thinking could be attributed to three mechanisms: attentional bias, interpretation bias or response bias. Therefore, there are three different stages in cognitive processing in which wishful thinking could arise.[4][13] First, at the lowest stage of cognitive processing, individuals selectively attend to cues. Individuals can attend to evidence that supports their desires and neglect contradictory evidence.[4][13] Second, wishful thinking could be generated by selective interpretation of cues. In this case, an individual is not changing their attention to the cue but the attribution of importance to the cue.[13] Finally, wishful thinking can arise at a higher stage of cognitive processing, such as when forming a response to the cue and inserting bias.[13]

Wishful seeing can be attributed to the same mechanisms as wishful thinking because it involves the processing of situational cues, including visual cues. However, with preconscious processing of visual cues and their associations with desirable outcomes, interpretation bias and response bias are not plausible since they occur in conscious cognitive processing stages.[14] Therefore, a fourth mechanism called perceptual set can also explain this phenomenon.[4] This mechanism proposes that mental states or associations activated before an object comes into view subtly guide the visual system during processing. Therefore, cues are readily recognized when they are related to such a mental state or association.[4]

Some speculate that wishful seeing results from cognitive penetrability in that higher cognitive functions are able to directly influence perceptual experience instead of only influencing perception at higher levels of processing. Those that argue against cognitive penetrability feel that sensory systems operate in a modular fashion with cognitive states exerting their influence only after the stimuli has been perceived.[8] The phenomenon of wishful seeing implicates cognitive penetrability in the perceptual experience.[4]

The extrastriate cortex (shown in orange and red) is believed to be involved in perceptual priming.

Wishful seeing has been observed to occur in early stages of categorization. Research using ambiguous figures and binocular rivalry exhibit this tendency.[15] Perception is influenced by both top-down and bottom-up processing. In visual processing, bottom-up processing is a rigid route compared to flexible top-down processing.[16] Within bottom-up processing, the stimuli are recognized by fixation points, proximity and focal areas to build objects, while top-down processing is more context sensitive. This effect can be observed via priming as well as with emotional states.[17] The traditional hierarchical models of information processing describe early visual processing as a one-way street: early visual processing goes into conceptual systems, but conceptual systems do not affect visual processes.[18] Currently, research rejects this model and suggests conceptual information can penetrate early visual processing rather than just biasing the perceptual systems. This occurrence is called conceptual or cognitive penetrability. Research on conceptual penetrability utilize stimuli of conceptual-category pairs and measure the reaction time to determine if the category effect influenced visual processing,[17] The category effect is the difference in reaction times within the pairs such as Bb to Bp. To test conceptual penetrability, there were simultaneous and sequential judgments of pairs. The reaction times decreased as the stimulus onset asynchrony increased, supporting categories affect visual representations and conceptual penetrability. Research with richer stimuli such as figures of cats and dogs allow for greater perceptual variability and analysis of stimulus typicality (cats and dogs were arranged in various positions, some more or less typical for recognition). Differentiating the pictures took longer when they were within the same category (doga-dogb) compared between categories (dog-cat) supporting category knowledge influences categorization. Therefore, visual processing measured by physical differential judgments is affected by non-visual processing supporting conceptual penetrability.[17]

Neural circuitry

[edit]

The areas of the brain that motivate wishful seeing and thinking are associated with the same regions that underlie social identification and reward. A study looked at these structures using MRI while participants estimated the winning probabilities for a series of football teams. Prior to this estimation, the individuals specified their favorite, neutral and least favorite NFL teams. Wishful thinking has been associated with social identity theory where individual seem to prefer in-group members over out-group members.[19] In this case, these individuals preferred the football team they most identified.

During wishful thinking tasks, differential activity was found in three areas of the brain: dorsal medial prefrontal cortex, the parietal lobe, and the fusiform gyrus in the occipital lobe. Differential activity in the occipital and parietal areas suggests a mode of selective attention to the cues presented; therefore, supporting a lower-level cognitive processing or attention bias.[14] However, differential activity in the prefrontal cortex also suggests higher-cognitive processing. The prefrontal cortex activity is related to preferences involved in social identification.[14] As a result, when cues are relevant to an individual, such as a favorite football team, the prefrontal cortex is activated. This identification of self carries hedonic value which in turn stimulates the reward system.[14] Differential activation of the reward system areas was only seen in conjunction with the activation of occipital lobe.[14] Thus, the activation of the reward system with an identification of self could lead to guidance of visual attention.[14][19]

Magnocellular (M) and parvocellular (P) pathways, which feed into the orbitofrontal cortex, play important roles in top-down processes that are susceptible to cognitive penetrability.[17] Magnocellular processing biased stimuli deferentially activates the orbitofrontal cortex; fast magnocellular projections link early visual and inferotemporal object recognition and work with the orbitofrontal cortex by helping generate early object predictions based on perceptual sets.[18] Stimuli were M-biased with low-luminance, achromatic line drawings or P-biased with isoluminate, chromatic line drawings and participants were asked if the drawing was larger or smaller than a shoebox.[18] Functional magnetic resonance imaging was used to monitor brain activity in the orbitofrontal cortex and ventrotemporal regions to determine which pathway aided faster object recognition.[18] The results supported that magnocellular neurons play a vital role in low-resolution object recognition as the neurons aid in quickly triggering top-down processes that provide initial guesses that lead to faster object recognition.[18]

Attention

[edit]
Focused attention

Humans have a physiologically limited visual field that must be selectively directed to certain stimuli. Attention is the cognitive process that allows this task to be accomplished and it might be responsible for the phenomenon of wishful seeing. Expectations, desires and fears are among the various factors that help direct attention.[9] Consequently, these cognitive experiences have the opportunity to influence the perceptual experience. In turn, attention can organize planned movement, providing a mechanism through which visual stimuli can influence behavior.[20]

Attentional deficits can also lead to altered perceptual experiences. Inattentional blindness, where unexpected events go by undetected, is one such deficit.[21] Using an inattentional blindness paradigm, researchers, White and Davies, had participants fixate on a cross in the center of the screen. First, a number cue denoting the number of letters that would appear on the arms of the cross appeared in the center of the cross. Following the cue, the actual letters would appear on the arms of the cross. Over four trials, the number of letters matched the number that was cued. On the fifth trial, half of the participants were cued to expect a smaller number of letters and half were cued to expect the correct number of letters. The letters then appeared on the screen accompanied by an unexpected stimulus. Participants were asked which letters appeared and whether they had seen any additional object. Participants cued to expect fewer letters were more susceptible to inattentional blindness as they failed to detect the unexpected stimulus more often than participants who had been cued to expect the correct number of stimuli. These results indicate that attentional capacity is affected by expectations.[22] This provides further evidence that cognitive processes converge in order to help construct the perceptual experience.

Although attention can lead to enhanced perceptual processing, the lack of attention to stimuli can also lead to a perceived enhanced perception of the stimuli.[23] Participants were pre-cued that indicated the diagonal to which they should be attending. They were then presented with stimuli (gratings with different textures) and then a response cue that indicated the diagonal for which the participants had to judge their perception. 70% of the time the response cue matched the pre-cue and 30% of the time did not match the pre-cue. The participants were asked to report the texture of the gratings that appeared in the response-cue and discriminate its visibility. This set-up allowed them to compare the perception of attended (cued) and non-attended stimuli (uncued).[23] Higher visibility was reported for stimuli that was unattended. Therefore, inattention lead to an overestimation of perception sensitivity.[23] This study suggests attention bias, a mechanism of wishful thinking, does not only rely on what individuals fixate upon, but the unattended stimuli as well.

Interpretation of emotion

[edit]

Emotion is often interpreted through visual cues on the face, body language and context.[24] However, context and cultural backgrounds have been shown to influence the visual perception and interpretation of emotion.[24][25] Cross-cultural differences in change blindness have been associated with perceptual set, or a tendency to attend to visual scenes in a particular way.[26] For example, eastern cultures tend to emphasize the background of an object, while western cultures focus on the central objects in a scene.[26] Perceptual sets are also the result of cultural aesthetic preferences. Therefore, cultural context can influence how people sample information from a face, just like they would do in a situational context. For example, Caucasians generally fixate around eyes, nose and mouth, while Asians fixate on eyes.[25] Individuals from different cultural backgrounds who were shown a series of faces and asked to sort them into piles in which every face showed the same emotion. Fixation on different features of the face leads to disparate reading of emotions.[25] Asians' focus on the eyes lead to the perception of startled faces as surprise rather than fear.[25] As a result, previous associations or customs of an individual can lead to different categorization or recognition of emotion. This particular difference in visual perception of emotion seems to suggest an attention bias mechanism for wishful seeing, since certain visual cues were attended to (e.g. nose, eyes) and the others were ignored (e.g. mouth).

Optimism

[edit]

Wishful seeing is also linked to optimism bias through which individuals tend to expect positive outcomes from events despite such expectations having little basis in reality. In order to determine the neural correlates underlying optimism bias, one functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) study imaged the brains of individuals as they recalled autobiographical moments related to life events and then rated their memories on several scales. These ratings revealed that participants viewed future positive events as more positive than past positive events and negative events as more temporally distant. The active brain regions, compared to a fixation point, were the rostral anterior cingulate cortex (rACC) and the right amygdala. Both of these areas became less active when imagining negative future events. The rACC is implicated in assessing emotional content and has strong connections to the amygdala. It is suggested that the rACC regulates activation in brain regions associated with emotion and autobiographical memory, thus allowing for the projection of positivity onto images of future events.[27]

It is important to consider physical aspects such as eye movement and brain activity and their relationship to wishful thinking, wishful seeing, and optimism. Isaacowitz (2006) investigated the motivational role of gaze, which he claims is highly correlated to an individual's interests and personality.[28] In his study, participants who embodied varying levels of self-reported optimism were directed to look at images of skin cancer, line drawings that were similar to the cancer pictures, and neutral faces.[28] Using a remote eye tracking system that measured the movement of the participants gaze, Isaacowitz found that more optimistically minded young adults gazed less on the skin cancer images when compared to the less optimistically minded participants.[28] This data was replicated in a follow-up study in which participants were screened for their genetically based risk for contracting skin cancer (even though some participants were more at risk than others, higher levels of optimism were correlated with a less fixated gaze on the skin cancer images despite the fact that the images were relevant to some participants).[28]

Methodology

[edit]

Wishful thinking is often studied in the context of psychology through the application of ambiguous figure studies, the hypothesis being that when presented with an ambiguous stimuli, the participant will interpret the stimuli in a certain way depending on the conditions or priming the participant experiences.

Balcetis and Dunning (2013) investigated wishful seeing by conducting two experiments, one involving two ambiguous stimuli that could be perceived as "B" or "13", and the other either a horse or a seal. The second experiment was a binocular rivalry test in which the participants were presented simultaneously with the letter "H" or number "4" (one stimuli in each eye). In each experiment, the experimenters associated one of the stimuli with desirable outcomes, and the other with a negative outcome (i.e. the "B" was associated with freshly squeezed orange juice while the "13" was associated with an undesirable health food smoothie, and in the binocular rivalry experiment, letters were associated with economic gain while numbers were associated with economic loss).[4] The results of the experiment demonstrated that participants were more likely to perceive the stimulus that was associated with a positive situation or outcome than the stimulus associated with negative situations.[4] This strong correlation between perception and positive versus negative stimuli demonstrates that we tend to see the world based on our own desires. The concept of wishful seeing hints towards a motivation-based perception process.[4]

Balcetis and Dale (2007) further considered that we view the world in biased ways in their four-prong study, one part of which addressed motivated object interpretation using a situation involving the interpretation of an ambiguous object (i.e. a Necker cube) that lacks the language based labels that the priming information may suggest to the participants. Many studies claim that what humans perceive or see is based on our internal motivation and goals, but it is important to consider that some priming situations in certain studies, or even the internal views of the participant, can affect the interpretation of a stimulus.[4] With these considerations in mind, Balcetis and Dale (2007), divided 124 Cornell University undergraduates into three groups which were each asked to imagine one of three detailed conditions: an upward-looking condition (participants were asked to imagine looking up at a large building), a downward-looking position (looking into a deep canyon), and a neutral/flat condition (standing in a flat field). The participants were then shown an ambiguous Necker cube on a computer screen and were told to click one of the two blue lines that seemed the closest to them. The line the participants chose depended on whether they determined the cube to be facing upwards or downwards.[29] The results of the study demonstrated that the majority of participants in the upward-looking condition saw the cube as facing upwards, the majority of downwards-looking conditioned patients saw the cube as facing downwards, and the participants in the neutral condition were evenly divided.[29] These results show that the priming stimulus language influenced object identification.[29] Motivation-affected object identification was observed in each condition.[29]

Similar results were seen in a study conducted by Changizi and Hall (2001), which addressed wishful thinking and goal-oriented object identification by investigating levels of thirst among participants in relation to their tendency to identify an ambiguously transparent stimulus as transparent (the study states that transparency is a natural yet unobvious quality directly related to water, a typically clear substance).[30] The results of the study showed a clear tendency for the thirsty participants (who were directed to eat a bag of potato chips immediately preceding the study) to interpret the ambiguous stimuli as transparent.[30] Furthermore, the participants who were not thirsty (they were directed to drink water before the study until they reported themselves as not thirsty) were less likely to interpret the ambiguous stimuli as transparent.[30] The study concludes that an alteration of a biological state, in this case the participants' level of thirstiness, that inspires wishful thinking, can directly affect the perception of visual stimuli.[30]

Bastardi, Uhlmann, and Ross (2011), showed the effects of wishful thinking when they presented parents with two fictional studies involving day care versus home care for their children. The parents who were conflicted (planned to use day care despite believing home care to be superior) more positively rated the "study" that claimed day care as superior and more negatively rated the study that claimed home care was better. The unconflicted parents (those that thought home care was superior to day care and planned to use only home care) rated the study that claimed home care was better more positively. The parents rated the studies that claimed what they actually planned for their children was the superior action, even though (in the case of the conflicted parents) the study may have been in opposition to their original beliefs.[1] In a post-experiment evaluation, the conflicted parents changed their initial beliefs and claimed to believe that home care was no better than day care, and the unconflicted parents continued to claim home care to be superior, though to a lesser degree.[1]

Balcetis and Dunning (2012) used the natural ambiguity found in judging distances to measure the effects of wishful seeing. During the study participants judged the distance to various stimuli while the experimenters manipulated the desirability of the stimuli. In one study, participants had their thirst intensified by consuming a large portion of their daily sodium intake or quenched by drinking to satiety. They were then asked to estimate the distance to a bottle of water. Those participants who were thirstier ranked the bottle of water as more desirable and viewed it as closer than less thirsty participants.[9] Another study performed by Balcetis and Dunning had participants estimate the distance to test results that contained either positive or negative feedback and to $100 gift cards that they had the possibility of either winning or not. Participants viewed the forms as closer when they contained positive feedback and the $100 gift cards as closer when there was a possibility that they might win them.[9] Balcetis and Dunning took into account the possible influence of positive mood by measuring creativity through a word creation task, and arousal by physiological markers.[9] The experimenters also eliminated reporter bias in one of their studies by having participants throw a beanbag toward a gift card adhered to the floor. Underthrowing the beanbag indicated that the participant perceived the gift card as closer, while overthrowing the beanbag indicated that the participant perceived the gift card as further away. Their results suggest that there is a positivity bias in the perception of distance.[9]

The relationship between distance perception and positivity may be more complicated than originally thought because context can also influence the distortion of perception. In fact, in threatening situations, positivity bias may be put aside to enable an appropriate response. In turn, the perceptual exaggerations brought on by threatening stimuli can be negated by psychosocial resources.[31] Psychosocial resources are defined by the Resources and Perception Model (RPM) as social support, self-worth, self- esteem, self-efficacy, hope, optimism, perceived control and self-disclosure. The participants reported distance measures while the experimenters manipulated the self-worth of the participants through mental imagery exercises, as well as their exposure to threatening (a tarantula) or non-threatening (a cat toy) stimuli. An effect of self-worth was only observed upon exposure to the threatening stimuli, when increased self-worth was correlated with a more realistic estimate of the distance to the threatening stimuli.[31]

Representations of environment

[edit]

Another common area in which wishful seeing can be observed is through environmental representations.[4] Many studies have supported that desire or motivations affect estimates of size, distance, speed, length and slope of environments or targets. For example, people will perceive desired objects as closer.[4] Wishful seeing also affects athletes' perception of balls and other equipment.[32] For example, softball players who see the ball as bigger, hit better; tennis players who return better, see the net as lower and the ball as moving slower.[32]

Distance and slope perception is affected by energy levels. Subjects with a heavier load see hills as steeper and distances as farther. Targets placed uphill seem farther away than those on flat ground. Fit people perceive hills as shallower while fatigued runners see hills as steeper.[4][33] This perception is modulated by what has been coined as "efficient energy expenditure."[34] In other words, perceived increase in effort (a steeper slope) when physically exhausted, might prompt individuals to rest rather than expend more energy.[33]

Distance perception is also affected by cognitive dissonance.[4] Cognitive dissonance was manipulated by high choice groups which were led to believe they chose to wear a Carmen Miranda outfit to walk across campus versus a low choice group which was told they had to wear the outfit. To reduce cognitive dissonance in high choice groups the subjects changed their attitude to match the situation. Thus, they perceived their environment in a less extreme way (shorter distance) than low choice groups.[35] Similar results followed with a perception of slope test, in which participants were in high and low choice groups to push themselves up a slope on skateboard with only their arms. Again, the high choice group perceived the slope as shallower than the low choice in order to reduce cognitive dissonance. Both of these studies suggest that intrapsychic motives play a role in perception of environments in order to encourage the perceiver to engage in behaviors that lead them either to acquire a desired object or be able to complete a desired task.[35]

Procrastination and motivation

[edit]

Sigall, Kruglanski, and Fyock (2000) found that people who were assessed to be high wishful thinkers were more likely to procrastinate when motivated to do so (by being told that the task they were about to do was unpleasant). When told the task was going to be pleasant, there was little difference in the amount of procrastination, showing that when motivated, wishful thinkers may consider themselves more capable of doing the task in a lesser amount of time, therefore exhibiting wishful thinking and considering themselves more capable than they are, and as a result, put off working on the unpleasant task.[36]

See also

[edit]

References

[edit]
  1. ^ a b c Bastardi, A.; Uhlmann, E. L.; Ross, L. (2011). "Wishful Thinking: Belief, Desire, and the Motivated Evaluation of Scientific Evidence". Psychological Science. 22 (6): 731–732. doi:10.1177/0956797611406447. PMID 21515736. S2CID 35422463.
  2. ^ Rosenthal, Robert; Jacobson, Lenore (1992). Pygmalion in the classroom : teacher expectation and pupils' intellectual development (Newly expanded ed.). Bancyfelin, Carmarthen, Wales: Crown House Pub. ISBN 978-1904424062.[page needed]
  3. ^ "The Pygmalion Effect". www.duq.edu. Duquesne University. Retrieved 12 November 2017.
  4. ^ a b c d e f g h i j k l m n o p q Dunning, D.; Balcetis, E. (2013). "Wishful Seeing: How Preferences Shape Visual Perception". Current Directions in Psychological. doi:10.1177/0963721412463693. S2CID 32186366.
  5. ^ Curtis, Gary. "Logical Fallacy: Wishful Thinking". fallacyfiles.org. Archived from the original on 2025-08-14. Retrieved 2025-08-14.
  6. ^ D'Arms, Justin; Jacobson, Daniel (2000). "The Moralistic Fallacy: On the 'Appropriateness' of Emotions". Philosophy and Phenomenological Research. 61 (1): 65–90. doi:10.2307/2653403. JSTOR 2653403.
  7. ^ Sabel, Bernhard A.; Wang, Jiaqi; Cárdenas-Morales, Lizbeth; Faiq, Muneeb; Heim, Christine (June 2018). "Mental stress as consequence and cause of vision loss: the dawn of psychosomatic ophthalmology for preventive and personalized medicine". EPMA Journal. 9 (2): 133–160. doi:10.1007/s13167-018-0136-8. PMC 5972137. PMID 29896314.
  8. ^ a b Stokes, D. (2011). "Perceiving and desiring: A new look at the cognitive penetrability of experience" (PDF). Philosophical Studies. 158 (3): 477–492. doi:10.1007/s11098-010-9688-8. S2CID 170806271.
  9. ^ a b c d e f g Riccio, M.; Cole, S.; Balcetis, E. (2013). "Seeing the Expected, the Desired, and the Feared: Influences on Perceptual Interpretation and Directed Attention". Social and Personality Psychology Compass. 7 (6): 401–414. doi:10.1111/spc3.12028.
  10. ^ Balcetis, E.; Dunning, D. (2009). "Wishful Seeing: More Desired Objects Are Seen as Closer". Psychological Science. 21 (1): 147–152. doi:10.1177/0956797609356283. PMID 20424036. S2CID 20223100.
  11. ^ Van Ulzen, N. R.; Semin, G. N. R.; Oudejans, R. U. R. D.; Beek, P. J. (2007). "Affective stimulus properties influence size perception and the Ebbinghaus illusion". Psychological Research. 72 (3): 304–310. doi:10.1007/s00426-007-0114-6. PMC 2668624. PMID 17410379.
  12. ^ Cole, S.; Balcetis, E.; Dunning, D. (2012). "Affective Signals of Threat Increase Perceived Proximity". Psychological Science. 24 (1): 34–40. doi:10.1177/0956797612446953. PMID 23160204. S2CID 18952116.
  13. ^ a b c d Krizan, Z.; Windschitl, P. D. (2007). "The influence of outcome desirability on optimism". Psychological Bulletin. 133 (1): 95–121. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.133.1.95. PMID 17201572. S2CID 14631800.
  14. ^ a b c d e f Aue, T.; Nusbaum, H. C.; Cacioppo, J. T. (2011). "Neural correlates of wishful thinking". Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience. 7 (8): 991–1000. doi:10.1093/scan/nsr081. PMC 3501709. PMID 22198967.
  15. ^ Balcetis, E.; Dunning, D. (2006). "See what you want to see: Motivational influences on visual perception". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 91 (4): 612–625. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.91.4.612. PMID 17014288.
  16. ^ Robinson-Riegler 2011, pp. 46–50.
  17. ^ a b c d Lupyan, G.; Thompson-Schill, S. L.; Swingley, D. (2010). "Conceptual Penetration of Visual Processing". Psychological Science. 21 (5): 682–691. doi:10.1177/0956797610366099. PMC 4152984. PMID 20483847.
  18. ^ a b c d e Kveraga, K.; Boshyan, J.; Bar, M. (2007). "Magnocellular Projections as the Trigger of Top-Down Facilitation in Recognition". Journal of Neuroscience. 27 (48): 13232–13240. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3481-07.2007. PMC 6673387. PMID 18045917.
  19. ^ a b Babad, E.; Katz, Y. (1991). "Wishful Thinking – Against All Odds". Journal of Applied Social Psychology. 21 (23): 1921–1938. doi:10.1111/j.1559-1816.1991.tb00514.x.
  20. ^ Wood, G.; Vine, S. J.; Wilson, M. R. (2013). "The impact of visual illusions on perception, action planning, and motor performance". Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics. 75 (5): 830–834. doi:10.3758/s13414-013-0489-y. PMID 23757046.
  21. ^ Robinson-Riegler 2011, pp. 99–101.
  22. ^ White, R. C.; Davies, A. A. (2008). "Attention set for number: Expectation and perceptual load in inattentional blindness". Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance. 34 (5): 1092–1107. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.34.5.1092. PMID 18823197.
  23. ^ a b c Rahnev, D.; Maniscalco, B.; Graves, T.; Huang, E.; De Lange, F. P.; Lau, H. (2011). "Attention induces conservative subjective biases in visual perception". Nature Neuroscience. 14 (12): 1513–1515. doi:10.1038/nn.2948. hdl:2066/115488. PMID 22019729. S2CID 205433966.
  24. ^ a b Barrett, L. F.; Kensinger, E. A. (2010). "Context is Routinely Encoded During Emotion Perception". Psychological Science. 21 (4): 595–599. doi:10.1177/0956797610363547. PMC 2878776. PMID 20424107.
  25. ^ a b c d Barrett, L. F.; Mesquita, B.; Gendron, M. (2011). "Context in Emotion Perception". Current Directions in Psychological Science. 20 (5): 286–290. doi:10.1177/0963721411422522. S2CID 35713636.
  26. ^ a b Robinson-Riegler 2011, pp. 101–102.
  27. ^ Sharot, T.; Riccardi, A. M.; Raio, C. M.; Phelps, E. A. (2007). "Neural mechanisms mediating optimism bias". Nature. 450 (7166): 102–5. Bibcode:2007Natur.450..102S. doi:10.1038/nature06280. PMID 17960136. S2CID 4332792.
  28. ^ a b c d Isaacowitz, D. M. (2006). "Motivated Gaze. The View from the Gazer" (PDF). Current Directions in Psychological Science. 15 (2): 68–72. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.136.9645. doi:10.1111/j.0963-7214.2006.00409.x. S2CID 16392932.
  29. ^ a b c d Balcetis, E.; Dale, R. (2007). "Conceptual set as a top – down constraint on visual object identification". Perception. 36 (4): 581–595. doi:10.1068/p5678. PMID 17564203. S2CID 15659017.
  30. ^ a b c d Changizi, M. A.; Hall, W. G. (2001). "Thirst modulates a perception". Perception. 30 (12): 1489–1497. doi:10.1068/p3266. PMID 11817755. S2CID 31335451.
  31. ^ a b Harber, K. D.; Yeung, D.; Iacovelli, A. (2011). "Psychosocial resources, threat, and the perception of distance and height: Support for the resources and perception model". Emotion. 11 (5): 1080–1090. doi:10.1037/a0023995. PMID 21707147.
  32. ^ a b Witt, J. K. (2011). "Action's Effect on Perception". Current Directions in Psychological Science. 20 (3): 201–206. doi:10.1177/0963721411408770. S2CID 393436.
  33. ^ a b Robinson-Riegler 2011, pp. 64–67.
  34. ^ Proffitt, D. R. (2006). "Embodied Perception and the Economy of Action". Perspectives on Psychological Science. 1 (2): 110–122. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.498.6331. doi:10.1111/j.1745-6916.2006.00008.x. PMID 26151466. S2CID 10991519.
  35. ^ a b Balcetis, E.; Dunning, D. (2007). "Cognitive Dissonance and the Perception of Natural Environments". Psychological Science. 18 (10): 917–21. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.02000.x. PMID 17894610. S2CID 5926930.
  36. ^ Sigall, H.; Kruglanski, A.; Fyock, J. (2000). "Wishful Thinking and Procrastination". Journal of Social Behavior & Personality. 15 (5): 283–296.

Further reading

[edit]
什么是代谢 蒲公英有什么作用和功效 soho是什么意思 什么是房颤 甲状腺结节是什么症状
妈妈是什么意思 3月份出生是什么星座 灿烂的近义词是什么 贫血检查查什么项目 巫师是什么意思
上当是什么意思 早餐吃什么有营养 kpl是什么意思 肌肉劳损吃什么药 皮癣是什么原因引起的
煮玉米加什么才会香甜 机场地勤是干什么的 喝什么水好啊 心脏支架不能吃什么 女人肾虚吃什么药调理
朗朗乾坤下一句是什么hcv9jop7ns2r.cn 酸菜鱼用什么鱼hcv8jop3ns0r.cn 钡餐检查能查出什么hcv8jop8ns5r.cn 667什么意思hcv9jop6ns5r.cn 心功能iv级是什么意思hcv9jop5ns7r.cn
防是什么生肖inbungee.com 1964年属什么的hcv8jop8ns0r.cn 肝弥漫性病变是什么意思hcv8jop3ns1r.cn 血稠吃什么食物好得快0735v.com 贫血的人来姨妈会有什么症状hcv7jop4ns6r.cn
心脏跳的慢吃什么好hcv7jop6ns0r.cn 芝士是什么做的qingzhougame.com 田七与三七有什么区别creativexi.com 起司是什么hcv8jop1ns5r.cn 水猴子是什么dayuxmw.com
肩袖损伤吃什么药hcv8jop5ns3r.cn 怜香惜玉是什么意思hcv8jop9ns2r.cn 33是什么意思hcv7jop4ns6r.cn 突然长胖很多是什么原因hcv9jop7ns1r.cn 沈阳有什么特产hcv7jop9ns9r.cn
百度